V I EW PO I N T A R T I C L E Conflicting health information: a critical research need Delesha M. Carpenter PhD, MSPH,* Lorie L. Geryk PhD, MPH,** Annie T. Chen MSIS,† Rebekah H. Nagler PhD,‡ Nathan F. Dieckmann PhD§ and Paul K. J. Han MD, MA, MPH¶ *Assistant Professor, **Postdoctoral Research Associate, Department of Pharmaceutical Outcomes and Policy, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Asheville, NC, †Assistant Professor, Department of Biomedical and Health Informatics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, ‡Assistant Professor, School of Journalism & Mass Communication, University of Minnesota, Min- neapolis, MN, §Assistant Professor, Decision Research, Oregon Health & Science, University Portland, OR, ¶Director, Center for Outcomes Research and Evaluation, Maine Medical Center, Portland, ME and Tufts University Clinical and Translational Sciences Institute, Boston, MA, USA Correspondence Delesha M. Carpenter, PhD, MSPH One University Heights CPO 2125, Asheville, NC 28804 USA E-mail: dmcarpenter@unc.edu Accepted for publication 22 November 2015 Keywords: conflicting information, decision-making, health Abstract Conflicting health information is increasing in amount and visibility, as evidenced most recently by the controversy surrounding the risks and benefits of childhood vaccinations. The mechanisms through which conflicting information affects individuals are poorly under- stood; thus, we are unprepared to help people process conflicting information when making important health decisions. In this view- point article, we describe this problem, summarize insights from the existing literature on the prevalence and effects of conflicting health information, and identify important knowledge gaps. We propose a working definition of conflicting health information and describe a conceptual typology to guide future research in this area. The typol- ogy classifies conflicting information according to four fundamental dimensions: the substantive issue under conflict, the number of con- flicting sources (multiplicity), the degree of evidence heterogeneity and the degree of temporal inconsistency. Conflicting health information is a growing problem worldwide, as evidenced by high- profile controversies surrounding childhood vaccination and numerous other health issues. Mass media have increased the visibility of such conflicting and often politically charged controversial health information,1 while a growing professional emphasis on involving individuals in health-care decisions has increased the exposure of patients and provi- ders to conflicting health information in clinical encounters.2 More than ever before, patients, providers, caregivers and policy mak- ers are expected to evaluate conflicting health information from different sources, judge whether the information is credible and decide how to respond. Helping people process and evaluate conflict- ing health information is an increasingly important need in health care, yet for several reasons, this need remains largely unmet. Studies of the prevalence, causes and effects of conflict- ing health information have been limited. Although behavioural research outside of the health-care domain has yielded insights on how individuals process and evaluate conflicting information, these insights have not been trans- lated to health care. Thus, evidence-based 1173ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd., 19, pp.1173–1182 This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. doi: 10.1111/hex.12438 strategies to help patients make sense of conflict- ing health information are lacking. The purpose of this viewpoint article was to highlight what is currently known about the prevalence, causes and effects of conflicting health information and to outline an organized approach for future research aimed at addressing current knowl- edge gaps. Conflicting health information: prevalence, causes and effects Evidence on the prevalence of conflicting health information is limited to the findings of a small number of studies that examine the perceptions of patients, physicians and the general public regarding health topics such as medications, can- cer screening and nutrition. Studies suggest that 18–80% of patients receive conflicting medica- tion information,3–7 while approximately 50– 75% of patients8–10 and providers11 perceive conflicting information about cancer-screening guidelines. Additionally, 72% of US adults report medium to high exposure to conflicting nutrition information.12 Evidence on the causes or sources of conflict- ing health information is also limited, although studies have begun to shed light on these issues. People may encounter conflicting information actively, while searching for health informa- tion,13 or passively, as recipients of unsolicited health advice.14 Conflicting health information may originate from professional and lay sources including the Internet, written materi- als15 and personal testimonials.4 One study that examined 15 different information sources found that physicians, media and the Internet were the most common patient-reported sources of conflicting medication information.4 The Internet16–18 and media12,19,20 in particular present a plethora of conflicting information on numerous health topics,16,17 from screening guidelines to vaccinations. Social media is an increasingly common forum for dialogue about health issues, and studying exposure to conflict- ing health information on social media and in other informal settings is increasingly important given the growing number of indi- viduals obtaining heath information through such channels.19 Conflicting health information also has sev- eral potential negative effects, perhaps the most important of which is confusion among patients and providers. When people encounter conflict- ing health information, they may have trouble deciding whom to trust and may defer to the source they deem most credible.20 Although con- flicting expert opinions about complex issues such as health-care problems are arguably natu- ral and expected, laypersons may perceive such conflict as evidence of intentional bias or expert incompetence.21 These beliefs, in turn, have been associated with lower intentions to engage in health behaviours for which there is clear scientific consensus (e.g. fruit/vegetable con- sumption).12 Empirical evidence suggests that conflicting information may also increase anxi- ety,22 heighten risk perceptions,23 decrease the ability of individuals to assess the reliability of information sources24 and reduce medica- tion adherence.3,4 When people encounter conflicting health information, they may also try to make sense of it using various strategies, including filtering out misinformation,25 seeking additional infor- mation from a health-care provider14 and developing more sophisticated strategies to appraise it.26 Because greater cognitive effort is required to process contradictory vs. congruent information, conflicting health information may increase individuals’ use of heuristics – or mental shortcuts – that may exacerbate cognitive biases or lead to errors in judgment.27 For example, they may only focus on one source of informa- tion, such as their doctor,28 and leave out other important elements,29 or attend to and use infor- mation that is easier to evaluate.30,31 In addition, the heightened uncertainty that arises from conflicting information may motivate peo- ple to choose information sources and interpretations that are most consistent with what they want to believe.32,33 Experimental work by Brewer and colleagues found that discordant genetic and standard test results for risk of recurrence of breast cancer did not significantly change women’s preferences for ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al.1174 chemotherapy.34 Conflicting information may also result in decision paralysis – leading people to simply do nothing at all (a response known as the status quo bias).35 Conflicting health information: a working definition Past research suggests that conflicting health information is common, increasing in volume and visibility, and deleterious in its psychologi- cal and behavioural effects; however, the existing evidence base on this problem is limited. The critical need moving forward is to address knowledge gaps on the prevalence, causes and effects of conflicting information, but a prerequi- site for such research is conceptual clarity on how conflicting health information should be defined. We propose defining conflicting health infor- mation in terms of health-related propositions. By ‘propositions’ we mean statements or asser- tions about a health-related issue. These propositions, furthermore, may pertain to health-related scientific evidence, interpretations of the evidence or recommendations and guideli- nes issued by experts or other individuals. Propositions may originate from either a single source or multiple information sources and may either be actively sought by an individual (e.g. through an Internet search to determine whether coffee is bad for your heart) or passively encoun- tered (e.g. through an overheard conversation about how coffee causes heart disease). ‘Conflicting health information’ can then be operationally defined as two or more health- related propositions that are logically inconsis- tent with one another. The Merriam-Webster dictionary defines the term ‘conflicting’ as ‘being in conflict, collision, or opposition’ or ‘incom- patible’.36 The defining feature of ‘conflicting’ in our definition is that the propositions are discrepant such that a person could not simulta- neously engage in or believe both propositions at once. For example, if two propositions dif- fered on one point, such as the recommended age to initiate mammography screening (age 40 or 50), a woman could not initiate screening at both age 40 or 50. Similarly, if a person found a proposition online that ‘coffee is bad for your heart’ and the person’s physician told him/her that ‘coffee is not bad for your heart’, the per- son could not simultaneously believe both propositions. Conflicting health information: a provisional conceptual typology Beyond defining the meaning of the phe- nomenon, conflicting health information can be classified according to four fundamental dimen- sions: the substantive issue under conflict, the number of conflicting sources (multiplicity), the degree of evidence heterogeneity and the degree of temporal inconsistency. In Table 1, we pre- sent text excerpts of conflicting propositions related to measles–mumps–rubella (MMR) vac- cinations to illustrate these four dimensions. The first dimension of conflicting health infor- mation is the issue of conflict or specific health topic for which conflicting information exists. Issue of conflict is important because people’s reactions to conflicting information may vary based on the topic. For example, people may have more negative reactions to conflicting information regarding vaccine risk than the schedule for receiving vaccinations. The second dimension is multiplicity, or the number of dif- ferent sources of conflicting information. This dimension is potentially important because the sheer number of sources of conflicting informa- tion may moderate its effects. For example, people may react differently if they encounter conflicting information about vaccines from a single source vs. multiple sources, and people’s negative reactions may increase or plateau as the number of conflicting sources increases. Evidence heterogeneity, the third dimension, is important because individuals may process conflicting information differently when it comes from homogenous sources, such as scientific studies, vs. heterogeneous sources, such as scientific studies and personal anecdotes. Using the vac- cine example, a clinician may react differently to conflicting vaccine safety information from two scientific journals than to conflicting informa- ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al. 1175 T a b le 1 C o n fl ic ti n g h e a lt h in fo rm a ti o n ty p o lo g y a p p li e d to m e a sl e s– m u m p s– ru b e ll a (M M R ) va cc in a ti o n s D im e n si o n D e fi n it io n C a se E xa m p le s o f C o n fl ic ti n g In fo rm a ti o n fo r th e S p e ci fi c P ro p o si ti o n : ‘I sh o u ld g e t m y ch il d va cc in a te d ’ Is su e o f co n fl ic t Th e sp e ci fi c h e a lt h to p ic (e .g . d ia g n o si s, p ro g n o si s, ca u se , tr e a tm e n t) fo r w h ic h co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n e xi st s V a cc in e sc h e d u le vs . va cc in e ri sk – M u lt ip li ci ty Th e n u m b e r o f so u rc e s th a t p ro vi d e s co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n a b o u t a h e a lt h is su e S in g le so u rc e : co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n e xi st s w it h in th e sa m e so u rc e A n A m a zo n d is cu ss io n fo ru m in cl u d e s co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n th re a d s. A m a zo n d is cu ss io n fo ru m :1 O n 1 7 Ju ly 2 0 1 0 , 8 :4 2 :1 1 P M P D T, b e n s_ m o m sa ys : ‘F o r th o se o f yo u w h o st il lb e li e ve th e M M R va cc in e is th e ca u se o f a u ti sm , p le a se re a d th is a rt ic le . Y o u w il lfi n d th a t th e st u d y li n k in g th e M M R va cc in e to a u ti sm w a s n o t o n ly sp o n so re d b y a p a rt y w h o w a n te d to li n k th e va cc in e to a u ti sm , b u t th e d o ct o r (D r. W a k e fi e ld ) w h o p e rf o rm e d th e st u d y st o o d to g a in a lo t fr o m a n o u tc o m e w h e re th e va cc in e w a s p ro ve n u n sa fe ’. O n 2 3 Ju ly 2 0 1 0 , 7 :4 7 :3 5 A M P D T, H o t h a n d s sa ys : ‘b la b la b la .. .. e xp la in to m e , th e n , h o w ch il d re n w h o a re n o rm a l a n d h e a lt h y g o d o w n th e tu b e s. .. .r ig h t a ft e r re ce iv in g (s ic ) a va cc in e ? Th o u sa n d s? (h a ir sp li tt e r) o f p a re n ts , w h o w it n e ss th is th e m se lv e s, ca n n o t b e w ro n g .. .s o rr y’ M u lt ip le so u rc e s: co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n e xi st s a cr o ss tw o o r m o re so u rc e s A g o ve rn m e n t a g e n cy a n d a p h ys ic ia n m a k e d if fe re n t re co m m e n d a ti o n s a b o u t th e M M R va cc in a ti o n . R e co m m e n d a ti o n s o f th e A C IP :5 1 ‘F o r th e p re ve n ti o n o f m e a sl e s, m u m p s, a n d ru b e ll a , va cc in a ti o n is re co m m e n d e d fo r p e rs o n s a g e d ≥1 2 m o n th s’ R e co m m e n d a ti o n fr o m D r. Te n p e n n y: 2 ‘T h e d e ci si o n to va cc in a te o r n o t is im p o rt a n t a n d co m p le x. P a re n ts m u st ta k e o n th e re sp o n si b il it y to se e k e n o u g h in fo rm a ti o n to m a k e a n e d u ca te d , in fo rm e d d e ci si o n ’ ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al.1176 T a b le 1 . C o n ti n u e d D im e n si o n D e fi n it io n C a se E xa m p le s o f C o n fl ic ti n g In fo rm a ti o n fo r th e S p e ci fi c P ro p o si ti o n : ‘I sh o u ld g e t m y ch il d va cc in a te d ’ E vi d e n ce h e te ro g e n e it y Th e e xt e n t to w h ic h th e co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n co m e s fr o m si m il a r o r d if fe ri n g e vi d e n ti a ry ty p e s H o m o g e n o u s: co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n fr o m si m il a r e vi d e n ti a ry ty p e s Tw o jo u rn a l a rt ic le s re p o rt d if fe re n t fi n d in g s a b o u t th e li n k b e tw e e n M M R va cc in a ti o n s a n d a u ti sm . Th e La n ce t (1 9 9 8 ): 3 8 ‘. .. th e o n se t o f [r e g re ss iv e d e ve lo p m e n ta ld is o rd e r] b e h a vi o u ra lp ro b le m s h a d b e e n li n k e d , e it h e r b y th e p a re n ts o r b y th e ch il d ’s p h ys ic ia n , w it h m e a sl e s, m u m p s, a n d ru b e ll a va cc in a ti o n ’ Th e La n ce t (1 9 9 9 ): 5 2 ‘O u r a n a ly se s d o n o t su p p o rt a ca u sa la ss o ci a ti o n b e tw e e n M M R va cc in e a n d a u ti sm ’ H e te ro g e n e o u s: co n fl ic ti n g in fo rm a ti o n fr o m d if fe ri n g e vi d e n ti a ry ty p e s C o n fl ic ti n g va cc in a ti o n in fo rm a ti o n fr o m a ta b lo id p u b li ca ti o n a n d a jo u rn a l a rt ic le D a il y M a il (t a b lo id p u b li ca ti o n ) (2 0 0 6 )5 3 : ‘N e w A m e ri ca n re se a rc h sh o w s th a t th e re co u ld b e a li n k b e tw e e n th e co n tr o ve rs ia l M M R tr ip le va cc in e a n d a u ti sm a n d b o w e l d is e a se in ch il d re n ’ A m e ri ca n Jo u rn a l o f P u b li c H e a lt h (j o u rn a la rt ic le ) (2 0 0 4 ): 5 4 ‘O n th e b a si s o f th e re su lt s o f th is re vi e w , th e G A C V S a g re e d a n d co n cl u d e d th a t th e re is n o e vi d e n ce fo r a ca u sa la ss o ci a ti o n b e tw e e n M M R va cc in e a n d a u ti sm o r a u ti st ic sp e ct ru m d is o rd e rs ’ Te m p o ra l in co n si st e n cy W h e th e r co n fl ic ti n g p ro p o si ti o n s a b o u t a n is su e e xi st a t th e sa m e p o in t in ti m e o r a t d if fe re n t p o in ts in ti m e S yn ch ro n o u s: C o n fl ic ti n g p ro p o si ti o n s th a t e xi st a t th e sa m e p o in t in ti m e C u rr e n t d iv e rg e n ce in th e e xi st in g cl in ic a l p ra ct ic e g u id e li n e s o n th e va cc in a ti o n sc h e d u le is su e d b y th e A C IP a n d N A C I R e co m m e n d a ti o n s o f th e A C IP :5 1 ‘C u rr e n tl y, A C IP re co m m e n d s 2 d o se s o f M M R va cc in e ro u ti n e ly fo r ch il d re n w it h th e fi rs t d o se a d m in is te re d a t a g e 1 2 th ro u g h 1 5 m o n th s a n d th e se co n d d o se a d m in is te re d a t a g e 4 th ro u g h 6 ye a rs b e fo re sc h o o le n tr y’ R e co m m e n d a ti o n s o f th e N A C I: 5 5 ‘N A C I re co m m e n d s th a t th e fi rs t d o se o f M M R V b e a d m in is te re d a t 1 2 –1 5 m o n th s o f a g e , a n d th e se co n d a t 1 8 m o n th s o r a t 4 –6 ye a rs o f a g e (p re sc h o o l) ’ A sy n ch ro n o u s: C o n fl ic t b e tw e e n o ld e r a n d n e w e r p ro p o si ti o n s Th e 1 9 8 9 A C IP st a te m e n t co m p a re d to th e 1 9 8 7 A C IP st a te m e n ts 2 9 D e ce m b e r 1 9 8 9 :5 6 Th e A C IP re vi se d re co m m e n d a ti o n s o n m e a sl e s p re ve n ti o n to in cl u d e a tw o -d o se sc h e d u le u si n g co m b in e d M M R va cc in e 1 9 8 7 : Th e A C IP re co m m e n d a ti o n in cl u d e d a o n e -d o se sc h e d u le M M R , m e a sl e s– m u m p s– ru b e ll a ; A C IP , A d vi so ry C o m m it te e o n Im m u n iz a ti o n P ra ct ic e s; N A C I, N a ti o n a l A d vi so ry co m m it te e o n Im m u n iz a ti o n ; G A C V S , G lo b a l A d vi so ry C o m m it te e o n V a cc in e S a fe ty . 1 A m a zo n d is cu ss io n fo ru m . M M R va cc in e D O E S N O T ca u se a u ti sm !! !. Ju ly 2 0 1 0 . A va il a b le a t: h tt p :/ /w w w .a m a zo n .c o m /f o ru m /p a re n ti n g ? _ e n co d in g = U TF 8 & cd Fo ru m = Fx 2 0 C 4 9 8 E K 5 JY 4 S & cd Th re a d = Tx 9 P Z W G Z L9 N Y Y J, a cc e ss e d 1 0 Fe b ru a ry 2 0 1 5 . 2 D rt e n p e n n y. co m . W h e n P a re n ts Q u e st io n V a cc in a ti o n [B lo g p o st ]. 2 0 1 3 [c it e d 2 0 1 5 ]. A va il a b le a t: h tt p :/ /d rt e n p e n n y. co m /w h e n -p a re n ts -q u e st io n -v a cc in a ti o n /. ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al. 1177 tion from a scientific journal and a patient testi- monial. Temporal inconsistency, the temporal relationship between conflicting propositions, is the final dimension. Conflicting information that is asynchronous (reflecting inconsistency between logical propositions separated by time) may produce different behavioural effects than conflicting information that is synchronous (reflecting inconsistency between logical proposi- tions that exist simultaneously). Importantly, this problem is common and inherent to the nor- mal advancement of scientific knowledge – a process in which lower quality evidence (e.g. from observational studies) is often supplanted by higher quality evidence [e.g. from randomized controlled trials (RCTs)]. An additional consideration for asynchronous conflicting information is the frequency with which conflicts arise; if there is less time between exposures to conflicting propositions, this may have a more negative impact on individuals than if there is a greater period of time between exposures to con- flicting propositions. For example, if MMR guidelines change yearly, this may more nega- tively impact trust in sources than if the guidelines change every 5 years. Our proposed conceptual typology focuses on properties of conflicting information itself; according to our definition, health-related propositions are either conflicting or not. How- ever, we recognize that perceptions of conflicting information, rather than the objective existence of conflicting information, are important deter- minants of people’s behavioural responses. These perceptions, in turn, are determined by numerous factors including the complexity of information. For example, individuals may easily recognize conflicting health propositions that directly oppose each other: ‘coffee is good for your heart’ and ‘coffee is bad for your heart’. In contrast, individuals may find it difficult to identify conflict in the propositions ‘coffee is bad for your heart’ and ‘coffee is bad for your heart if you drink more than one cup per day’. Addi- tionally, the personal salience of information may influence the degree to which individuals perceive informational conflict. Using the coffee example, someone who does not drink coffee may be less likely to recognize conflicting propo- sitions about coffee. Several other factors may cause individuals to perceive informational conflict when it does not exist. For example, a person may perceive the absence of information as conflicting information if he/she experiences a rare medica- tion side-effect of which he/she was never informed. Individuals may also perceive infor- mational conflict when confronted by logically consistent propositions that have different health implications. For example, a person exposed to the propositions ‘coffee is bad for your heart’ and ‘coffee prevents Type II diabetes’ may per- ceive a conflict about whether to drink coffee, given that coffee drinking has both positive and negative health outcomes. However, such con- flict is decisional rather than informational, because the propositions themselves are not logi- cally inconsistent. Our operational definition focuses on informational conflict resulting from logical inconsistency in alternative health propo- sitions, rather than decisional conflict resulting from competing pros and cons of alternative choice options. Therefore, although the coffee and diabetes example presented above would not constitute an instance of informational con- flict, the causes and mechanisms underlying individuals’ subjective perceptions of conflict and how subjective perceptions affect health decisions are important areas for future research. Apparent conflicts in information may also result from differences in the particular out- comes that are reported in the coverage of research studies. For example, one news story may focus on a surrogate outcome, such as pre- vention of heart attacks, and report a significant benefit of a drug, while other reports may focus on cardiovascular disease mortality and report no benefit. Although these reporting differences may reflect expert disagreements over which out- comes ought to be publicized, they do not reflect true conflicts in the information per se. Finally, the nature of the decision at hand as well as the background of the individual making the deci- sion may determine the extent to which a person perceives informational conflict. For example, a patient who must make a decision for which ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al.1178 conflicting information exists – for example to undergo prostate cancer screening with the prostate-specific antigen test – may perceive informational conflict in scientific knowledge, while a researcher (whose goal is to simply pro- duce and evaluate the existing knowledge) may simply perceive available scientific evidence as being of limited quantity and/or heterogeneous quality, and not necessarily ‘conflicting’. At times, informational conflict may also reflect the varying strength of scientific evidence and thus may be more perceived than real. Observational and experimental studies may yield seemingly conflicting results that can take years of research and additional studies to reconcile. Individuals trained in research methodology understand that observational studies yield weaker evidence – which needs to be viewed sceptically – while RCTs yield stronger evidence that can invalidate weaker evi- dence. Thus, for a scientifically trained audience, apparent inconsistencies in the results of obser- vational and RCT findings may not necessarily be perceived as ‘conflicting information’. Lim- ited scientific literacy, however, may prevent the general public from understanding the hierarchy of evidence and the differences between correla- tional and causal studies; as a result, they may simply perceive such inconsistencies in evidence as conflicts between equivalent forms of evi- dence. For example, the public may perceive conflict between a cross-sectional survey of 50 000 people that finds that coffee is associated with a lower risk of heart disease and an RCT with 2500 participants that finds no association. In contrast, nutritional epidemiologists would regard these studies as non-comparable from an evidentiary standpoint and prioritize the RCT findings. Failure to understand the hier- archy of scientific evidence may also explain why attempts to educate the public about RCTs that found no evidence of a vaccina- tion–autism link have not made much headway in diffusing the negative effects of conflicting information.37 Fraudulent scientific studies can also produce conflicting information – and perpetuate percep- tions of conflict – among professional communities as well as the general public. The vaccination example given in Table 1 offers an excellent example of this. In 1998, Wakefield and colleagues published a paper in which they claimed that environmental triggers (i.e. the MMR vaccination) were associated with gastrointestinal disease and developmental regression (i.e. autism) in eight of 12 children studied.38 In 2010, twelve years later, the paper was officially retracted due to ethical misconduct and falsified data.39 In 2011, detailed informa- tion regarding why the findings on the link between autism and MMR vaccination were fraudulent was published in BMJ.40 Unfortu- nately, during the 12 years before the paper was retracted, the findings were disseminated in media and online outlets and served as a basis for the modern antivaccination movement.41 In fact, one study found that half of antivaccina- tion websites applaud doctors such as Wakefield for speaking out against vaccinations.41 Despite repeated and large-scale efforts to educate the general public about why MMR vaccination does not cause autism, the antivaccination movement remains strong and continues to cite Wakefield’s retracted study. Future research directions Many gaps exist in our understanding of con- flicting health information. Thus, we are not well-positioned to help patients and clinicians manage this growing problem. Moving forward, we believe a coherent, comprehensive pro- gramme of research is needed to determine the prevalence, causes and effects of conflicting health information and to develop effective strategies to deal with it. The multifaceted nat- ure of this phenomenon and its potential effects calls for a transdisciplinary programme of research. However, both the challenge and pro- mise of employing a transdisciplinary team of scientists (e.g. communication scholars, beha- vioural decision theorists, health services researchers) are that they have their own well- developed theories of how people process infor- mation and make decisions. These theories may provide useful lenses for understanding the ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al. 1179 causes and effects of conflicting information; however, exactly how to optimally integrate these theories is unclear and represents a funda- mental research need. For example, theories from the persuasion literature, including extended parallel process model42 and the uni- model43,44, may offer guidance as to how subjective perceptions (e.g. perceived threat) may influence processing of conflicting health information. Additionally, other theories, such as fuzzy trace theory,45 are applicable to the study of conflicting health information because they identify factors (e.g. low health literacy) that may interfere with information processing. Finally, theosries from other fields, such as arti- ficial intelligence, offer potentially useful insights on how people process and integrate conflicting information.46–48 Equally important is the need to develop valid and reliable measures of conflicting health infor- mation. Some measures exist,4,49,50 but more work is needed to assess their psychometric properties and to develop measures that accu- rately capture conflicting health information in its many potential manifestations. This initial psychometric work could then enable basic research aimed at elucidating the effects of con- flicting health information on health judgments and decisions and the mechanisms underlying these effects. Additional measurement work should be devoted to developing instruments that capture the intrapersonal factors (e.g. beliefs, information needs, perceived salience of topic) that are likely associated with how indi- viduals process conflicting information. Understanding the effects of conflicting health information would ultimately pave the way for applied research aimed at translating insights on the causes and effects of conflicting health infor- mation into interventions to help individuals better manage this information. We have pro- posed several key issues for consideration; however, there are additional issues related to study design and data analysis that could perpet- uate conflicting information. For example, a single study could result in conflicting informa- tion if researchers use alternative statistical methodologies that result in different interpreta- tions of the data. The fundamental prerequisite for a comprehensive programme of research on conflicting health information, however, is recognition of the phenomenon’s importance and conceptual clarity about what it entails. This viewpoint article is offered as a preliminary step towards these goals. Sources of funding Financial support for this research was provided in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health. The funding agreement ensured the authors’ independence in designing the research, interpreting the data, writing and publishing the report. Acknowledgements DMC’s salary was partially supported by the National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant KL2TR000084. RHN acknowledges sup- port from the Building Interdisciplinary Research Careers in Women’s Health Grant (2K12-HD055887) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institutes of Child Health and Human Development, the Office of Research on Women’s Health and the National Institute on Aging, administered by the University of Min- nesota Deborah E. Powell Center for Women’s Health. NFD acknowledges the generous sup- port of the U. S. National Science Foundation (Award#1231231 administered by Decision Research). PKJH acknowledges the support of the National Human Genome Research Institute under the Intergovernmental Personnel Act. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Science Foun- dation or the National Institutes of Health. Conflict of interests The authors have no conflict of interests to disclose. ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al.1180 References 1 Nagler RH, Fowler EF, Gollust SE. Covering controversy: what are the implications for women’s health?Women’s Health Issues, 2015; 25: 318–321. 2 Frosch DL, Moulton BW, Wexler RM et al. Shared decision making in the United States: policy and implementation activity on multiple fronts. Zeitschrift f€ur Evidenz, Fortbildung und Qualit€at im Gesundheitswesen, 2011; 105: 305–312. 3 Carpenter DM, DeVellis RF, Fisher EB et al. The effect of conflicting medication information and physician support on medication adherence for chronically ill patients. Patient Education and Counseling, 2010; 81: 169–176. 4 Carpenter DM, Elstad EA, Blalock SJ, DeVellis RF. Conflicting medication information: prevalence, sources, and relationship to medication adherence. Journal of Health Communication, 2014; 19: 67–81. 5 Blendon RJ, Schoen C, DesRoches C, Osborn R, Zapert K. Common concerns amid diverse systems: health care experiences in five countries.Health Affairs, 2003; 22: 106–121. 6 Lim AYN, Ellis C, Brooksby A, Gaffney K. Patient satisfaction with rheumatology practitioner clinics: can we achieve concordance by meeting patients’ information needs and encouraging participatory decision making? Annals of the Academy of Medicine, Singapore, 2007; 36: 110–114. 7 H€ameen-Anttila K, Nordeng H, Kokki E et al. Multiple information sources and consequences of conflicting information about medicine use during pregnancy: a multinational Internet-based survey. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 2014; 16: e60. 8 Squiers LB, Holden DJ, Dolina SE et al. The public’s response to the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force’s 2009 recommendations on mammography screening. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 2011; 40: 497–504. 9 Taplin SH, Urban N, Taylor VM, Savarino J. Conflicting national recommendations and the use of screening mammography: does the physician’s recommendation matter? The Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, 1997; 10: 88–95. 10 Niederdeppe J, Levy AG. Fatalistic beliefs about cancer prevention and three prevention behaviors. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention, 2007; 16: 998–1003. 11 Han PK, Klabunde CN, Breen N et al.Multiple clinical practice guidelines for breast and cervical cancer screening: perceptions of US primary care physicians.Medical Care, 2011; 49: 139–148. 12 Nagler RH. Adverse outcomes associated with media exposure to contradictory nutrition messages. Journal of Health Communication, 2014; 19: 24–40. 13 Eysenbach G. The impact of the Internet on cancer outcomes. CA: A Cancer Journal for Clinicians, 2003; 53: 356–371. 14 Elstad E, Carpenter DM, Devellis RF, Blalock SJ. Patient decision making in the face of conflicting medi- cation information. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 2012; 7: 1–11. 15 Vidal L, Shavit M, Fraser A, Paul M, Lleibovici L. Systematic comparison of four sources of drug information regarding adjustment of dose for renal function. BMJ, 2005; 331: 263–265. 16 Mashiach R, Seidman GI, Seidman DS. Use of mifepristone as an example of conflicting and misleading medical information on the internet. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 2002; 109: 437–442. 17 Mankoff J, Kuksenok K, Kiesler S, Rode JA, Waldman K. Competing online viewpoints and models of chronic illness. Paper presented at: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 2011. 18 Hwang KO, Farheen K, Johnson CW et al. Quality of weight loss advice on internet forums. The American Journal of Medicine, 2007; 120: 604–609. 19 Fox S. The social life of health information 2011. Pew Internet & American Life Project. Washington, DC, 2011. Available at: http://www.pewinternet.org/2011/ 05/12/the-social-life-of-health-information-2011/. 20 Hilton S, PetticrewM, Hunt K. Parents’ champions vs. vested interests: who do parents believe about MMR? a qualitative study. BMC Public Health, 2007; 7: 42. 21 Shanteau J. Why do experts disagree? In: Green B (ed.), Risk Behaviour and Risk Management in Business Life. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press, 2000: 186–196. 22 Pollock K, Grime J, Baker E, Mantala K. Meeting the information needs of psychiatric inpatients: staff and patient perspectives. Journal of Mental Health, 2004; 13: 389–401. 23 Han PKJ, Moser RP, Klein WMP. Perceived ambiguity about cancer prevention recommendations: relationship to perceptions of cancer preventability, risk, and worry. Journal of Health Communication, 2006; 11(1S): 51–69. 24 McIntosh A, Shaw CF. Barriers to patient information provision in primary care: patients’ and general practitioners’ experiences and expectations of information for low back pain.Health Expectations, 2003; 6: 19–29. 25 Given LM, Ruecker S, Simpson H, Sadler EB, Ruskin A. Inclusive interface design for seniors: image-browsing for a health information context. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 2007; 58: 1610–1617. 26 Kienhues D, Stadtler M, Bromme R. Dealing with conflicting or consistent medical information on the ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al. 1181 Web: when expert information breeds laypersons’ doubts about experts. Learning and Instruction, 2011; 21: 193–204. 27 Hibbard JH, Slovic P, Jewett JJ. Informing consumer decisions in health care: implications from decision- making research. The Milbank Quarterly, 1997; 75: 395–414. 28 Wegwarth O, Gigerenzer G. Trust-Your-Doctor: A Simple Heuristic in Need of a Proper Social Environment. Simple Heuristics in a Social World. New York: Oxford University Press, 2013. 29 Tversky A, Sattath S, Slovic P. Contingent weighting in judgment and choice. Psychological Review, 1988; 95: 371. 30 Hsee CK. Elastic justification: how tempting but task-irrelevant factors influence decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 1995; 62: 330–337. 31 Han PK. Conceptual, methodological, and ethical problems in communicating uncertainty in clinical evidence.Medical Care Research and Review, 2013; 70 (1S): 14S–36S. 32 Kunda Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 1990; 108: 480–498. 33 Keselman A, Browne AC, Kaufman DR. Consumer health information seeking as hypothesis testing. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 2008; 15: 484–495. 34 Brewer NT, Edwards AS, O’Neill SC et al.When genomic and standard test results diverge: implications for breast cancer patients’ preference for chemotherapy. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 2009; 117: 25–29. 35 Samuelson W, Zeckhauser R. Status quo bias in decision making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1988; 1: 7–59. 36 Merriam-Webster.com. conflicting. 2014. In Merriam-Webster.com. Retrieved July 14, 2014, from http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/ conflicting 37 Kahan DM. A risky science communication environment for vaccines. Science, 2013; 342: 53–54. 38 Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A et al. RETRACTED: ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. The Lancet, 1998; 351: 637–641. 39 Caplan AL. Retraction – ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. The Weekly Epidemiological Record, 2009; 84: 301–308. 40 Deer B. How the case against the MMR vaccine was fixed. BMJ, 2011; 342: c5347. 41 Kata A. A postmodern Pandora’s box: anti- vaccination misinformation on the Internet. Vaccine, 2010; 28: 1709–1716. 42 Witte K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Communications Monographs, 1992; 59: 329–349. 43 Kruglanski AW, Chen X, Pierro A et al. Persuasion according to the unimodel: implications for cancer communication. Journal of Communication, 2006; 56 (s1): S105–S122. 44 Kruglanski AW, Thompson EP. Persuasion by a single route: a view from the unimodel. Psychological Inquiry, 1999; 10: 83–109. 45 Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. Fuzzy-trace theory: an interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 1995; 7: 1–75. 46 Benferhat S, Kaci S, Le Berre D, Williams M-A. Weakening conflicting information for iterated revision and knowledge integration. Artificial Intelligence, 2004; 153: 339–371. 47 Lin J. Integration of weighted knowledge bases. Artificial Intelligence, 1996; 83: 363–378. 48 Cantwell J. Resolving conflicting information. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 1998; 7: 191–220. 49 Nagler RH, Hornik RC. Measuring media exposure to contradictory health information: a comparative analysis of four potential measures. Communication Methods and Measures, 2012; 6: 56–75. 50 Han PK, Reeve BB, Moser RP, Klein WM. Aversion to ambiguity regarding medical tests and treatments: measurement, prevalence, and relationship to sociodemographic factors. Journal of Health Communication, 2009; 14: 556–572. 51 McLean HQ, Fiebelkorn AP, Temte JL, Wallace GS. Prevention of measles, rubella, congenital rubella syndrome, and mumps, 2013: summary recommendations of the Advisory Committee on Immunization Practices (ACIP).MMWR - Recommendations and Reports, 2013; 62: 1–34. 52 Taylor B, Miller E, Farrington C et al. Autism and measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine: no epidemiological evidence for a causal association. The Lancet, 1999; 353: 2026–2029. 53 Beck S. The Daily Mail. Scientists fear MMR link to autism. Available at: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/ news/article-388051/Scientists-fear-MMR-link- autism.html, accessed 28 May 2006. 54 Folb PI, Bernatowska E, Chen R et al.A global perspective on vaccine safety and public health: the Global Advisory Committee on Vaccine Safety. American Journal of Public Health, 2004; 94: 1926–1931. 55 Tan B, Shainoor I. Statement onMeasles-Mumps- Rubella-Varicella Vaccine. National Advisory Committee on Immunization (NACI).Canada Communicable Disease Report, 2010; 36ACS-9: 1–22. 56 ACIP. Measles prevention: recommendations of the Immunization Practices Advisory Committee (ACIP). Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 1989, 38(S9); 1–18. ª 2015 The Authors. Health Expectations Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd Health Expectations, 19, pp.1173–1182 Conflicting health information, D M Carpenter et al.1182