RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY OF THE ADOLESCENT DISSOCIATIVE EXPERIENCES SCALE Steven R. Smith, B.A. Eve B. Carlson, Ph.D. Steven R. Smith, B.A., is a graduate student in clinical psy- chology at the University of Arkansas in Fayetteville, Arkansas. Eve B. Carlson, Ph.D., is a Clinical and Consulting Psychologist in Delevan, Wisconsin. For reprints write Steven R. Smith, B.A., Department of Psychology, University of Arkansas, 316 Memorial Hall, Fayetteville, AR, 72701. ABSTRACT The Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale (A-DES) is designed to measure dissociation in adolescents (ages 11-17). The A-DES mea- sures dissociation in four areas: dissociative amnesia, absorption and imaginative involvement, depersonalization and derealization, and passive influence. The present study was designed to establish A-DES norms for general population adolescents and to study aspects of the reliability and validity of the A-DES. Test-retest reliability was studied by testing one group of subjects twice, with a two-week inter- val between test administrations. Internal consistency was assessed by measuring the split-half reliability of the A-DES. Cronbach 's alphas were calculated for the A-DES global score and four subscales. Results of these three analyses provide evidence for the reliability of the A- DES. The concurrent validity of the A-DES was studied by correlat- ing scores on the A-DES with scores on the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) in a college sample. Results showed a high correlation between scores on these two measures. Overall, results indicate that the A-DES appears to have promise as a measure of dissociation in adolescents. Dissociation can be defined as a lack of integration of thoughts, feelings, and experiences into the normal stream of consciousness (Bernstein Putnam, 1986). Four cate- gories of dissociation have been identified and described by Putnam (1994) including memory dysfunctions, distur- bances in identity, passive influence, and absorption. Dissociative memory dysfunctions can take the form of amne- sia for events, intrusive memories, or "flashbacks" (vivid reliv- ing of an experience) . Dissociative memory dysfunctions can also include phenomena such as the inability to determine if a perceived memory represents an actual event or infor- mation obtained by reading, hearing, or thinking about the event. Disturbances in identity include feelings of being more than one person (dissociated identity), distortions in the per- ceptions of one own body (depersonalization), and the inability to remember important personal information (dis- sociative amnesia). Passive influence involves a feeling that one behaviors are caused by a force from within. Absorption refers to a very intense focusing of attention (e.g., becom- ing so engrossed in a television program or a movie that out- side events are not attended to or not perceived). Dissociative experiences happen to everyone, but vary in nature, severity, and frequency across different popula- tions (Bernstein Putnam, 1986). Most of these experiences in the general population are relatively infrequent and do not cause any disruption in normal functioning. Such nor- mative dissociative experiences arc minor and might include absorption experiences such as daydreaming. For persons with dissociative disorders, dissociative symptoms are much more frequent and severe than they are for those in the general population (Carlson Putnam, 1993). In these individuals, dissociative experiences occur so often as to disrupt their lives. Examples of symptoms found in persons with dissociative disorders include such experi- ences as having more than one distinct personality state or experiencing extended periods of amnesia for traumatic life events (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Among normative adolescent populations, dissociative experiences are more common than in normative adult pop- ulations (Putnam, 1991). Adolescents engage in behaviors such as intricate daydreaming and fantasizing that are dis- sociative in nature and might he considered disordered in adults (Putnam, 1994). However in adolescents, these behav- iors are a part of normal conscious processes inherent in the development of identity. During this pivotal time in life, chil- dren and adolescents fantasize about, experiment with, and create new aspects of themselves (Putnam, 1994). Manyado- lescents undergo sudden changes in identity and behavior and often feel divided into different versions of themselves. These differing identities are often conflictual and situa- tionally varying. Persons with a history of psychological trauma such as war, natural disasters, rape, or the witnessing of loss of life also experience more severe and frequent dissociation. (Classen, Koopman, Spiegel, 1993). Results of several stud- ies have found a relationship between severity of dissocia- 125 DISSOCIATION, Vol. IX. No. ?June 1996 THE RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY OF THE A-DES tion and the severity and frequency of traumatic experiences (American Psychiatric Association, 1987; Briere, 1988; Briere Runtz, 1988; Carlson, Armstrong, Loewenstein, Roth, in press; Chu Dill, 1990; Coons, Bowman, Pellow, Schneider, 1989; DiTomasso Routh, 1993; Goodwin, Cheeves, Connell, 1990; Kirby, Chu, and Dill 1993; Nash, Hulsey, Sexton, Harralson, Lambert 1993; Puntam, 1985; Strick Wilcoxon, 1991; Swett Halpert, 1993; van der Kolk, 1987; van der Kolk Kadish, 1987). A relationship between sexual and physical abuse his- tories and increased dissociation has also been found in a non-clinical sample of college-age adolescents (Sandberg Lynn, 1992). Results of this study showed that those report- ing histories of abuse or victimization were more dissocia- tive than those reporting no abuse. Evidence of a relationship between traumatic experi- ences and dissociation has also been found in adolescent sub- jects. In a study focusing on the association between trauma and dissociation in inpatient adolescents, physical and/or sexual abuse and psychological abuse were found to be sig- nificantly related to increased levels of dissociative symp- tomatology (Sanders Giolas, 1991). Unfortunately, these results are not conclusive as dissociation was measured using the DES, which was not designed for use with adoles- cents. In order to empirically study dissociation in adolescents, their dissociative experiences must be quantified with a mea- sure that is appropriate for use with adolescents. The mea- surement of dissociation in adolescents is important for two reasons. First, because high levels of dissociative symptoma- tology are related to experiences of trauma and stress, any individuals who experience above average amounts of dis- sociation may have had traumatic experiences. A measure of dissociation appropriate for adolescents might aid in the identification of traumatized individuals in clinical settings and might facilitate diagnosis and treatment (Carlson Armstrong, 1994) . Second, because dissociation is an impor- tant part of normative adolescent identity formation (Putnam, 1994), measurement of the frequency and inten- sity of the dissociation and of different subtypes of dissocia- tion may be useful in the examination of the different facets of adolescent emotional and cognitive development. Measures have been developed for the quantification of dissociation in adults and children. The Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) was developed for use as a measure of dissociation in adult populations (Bernstein Putnam, 1986; Carlson Putnam, 1993). Several dissociation scales also exist for child populations, of which the Child Dissociative Checklist is the most widely used (Putnam, Helmers, Trickett, 1993) . Both of these measures are valid and reliable screening instruments for dissociation. However, the DES is not appropriate for use with persons under the age of 18 because the language and experiences described in the DES would be inappropriate to younger subjects. The 126 CDC is not appropriate for adolescents as many of its items do not apply to adolescents. Moreover, because the observ- er-scored method of the CDC quantifies specific observ behaviors, it does not measure cognitive or emotional dis- sociation without behavioral indicators. For this reason, the CDC is not sensitive to some of dissociative symptoms of inter- est. Until now, there has been no measure of dissociation specifically for adolescents. Recently the Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale was developed to mea- sure dissociation in children between the ages of 11 and 17 (Armstrong, Putnam, Carlson, Libero, Smith, 1997). The A-DES is a 30-item self-report measure designed to assess dis- sociation in four areas: dissociative amnesia, passive influ- ence, depersonalization and derealization, and absorption and imaginative involvement. Armstrong et al. (1997) examined the relationship between A-DES scores and traumatic histories in adolescents referred for psychological evaluation. They found that A-DES scores for non-abused adolescents were significantly lower than scores for adolescents who reported both physical and sexual abuse histories. Furthermore, their study examined the ability of A-DES scores to differentiate adolescents with dissociative disorders from those with other diagnoses. They found that adolescents with dissociative disorders scored sig- nificantly higher than adolescents in other diagnostic groups, with the exception of psychotic disorders. These findings sup- port the validity of the scale as a measure of dissociative expe- rience. The purpose of the present study was to establish pre- liminary A-DES norms using a non-clinical sample of subjects and to investigate the reliability and concurrent validity of the A-DES. First, we sought to establish norms for frequency and degree of dissociation across three age groups in junior and high school adolescents. We obtained A-DES subscale scores for these subjects as well as global A-DES scores. Furthermore, we sought to assess the reliability of the A- DES. Reliability refers to the consistency of scale scores over time as well as the extent to which a measure is internally consistent (Anastasi, 1988). In the present study, test-retest reliability and internal consistency were assessed. In addition to reliability, evidence for the constructvalid- ity of the A-DES was also assessed. Construct validity refers to the extent to which a measure accurately quantifies the con- struct that it was designed to assess (Anastasi, 1988). Concurrent validity was assessed by correlating DES and A- DES scores of college students. METHODS Participants Junior and senior high school subjects were from racial- ly and socioeconomically diverse schools in a small Midwestern city. Data was collected from students in three DISSOCIATION, Vol. IN, No. 2, June 1996 SMITH/CARLSO TABLE 1 A-DES Total and Subscale Means by Age AGE GROUP 12-13 14-15 16-17 College All Ssa (N=18) (N=22) (N=20) (N=46) (N=60) A-DES Total Score 2.33 (1.5) 2.14 (1.2) 2.26 (1.5) 0.78 (0.95) 2.24 (1.4) Amnesia 2.70 0.9) 2.14 (1.4) 2.35 (1.8) 1.38(1.15) 2.37 (1.7) Absorption 2.71 (1.6) 2.32 (1.6) 2.41 (1.5) 2.02 (1.20) 2.46 (1.6) Deper/Dereal 1.93 (1.8) 1.81 (1.1) 1.87 (1.7) 1.01(0.9) 1.87 (1.5) Passive Influence 2.57 (1.7) 2.74 (1.8) 2.92 (2.0) 1.71(1.24) 2.75 (1.8) Note: a College subject data not included. L age groups: 12 to 13 year-olds (N = 18, mean age = 12.6), 14 to 15 year-olds (N = 22, mean age = 14.7), 16 to 17 year-olds (N = 20, mean age = 16.4). These junior and senior high school students were selected from regular-level English class- es in public schools, in order to maximize the likelihood that they represented a cross-section of the entire student body in terms of race, sex, and socioeconomic status. Seventy-eight percent of the sample was Caucasian, 5% were African American, 3% were Native American, 5% were Asian, 3% were Hispanic, and 5% described their race as" Other." Sixty- two percent of the sample was male. A sample of college stu- dents from a small, Midwestern college located in a suburb of a mid-sized city was also tested. The students ranged in age from 18 to 21 years old (N = 46, mean age = 19.5). Materials The A-DES is a thirty-item self-report measure. Each item presents a statement in first person form (e.g., "My body feels as if it doesn belong to me."). Under each of these state- ments, subjects mark the frequency of these experiences on a scale from 0 to 10 with 0 labeled " never" and 10 labeled "always." Flesch-Kincaid and Coleman-Liau indices were cal- culated using Microsoft Word Version 6.0c (1994) comput- er software. The Flesch-Kincaid grade level readability index is 5.2, while the Coleman-Liau grade level index is 5.4. In a clinical sample, Armstrong et al. (1997) found the A-DES to have good internal reliability as indicated by a Spearman- Brown of r = .92 (p < .00001) and Cronbach alpha for the total scale score of r = .93 (p < .05). Total A-DES scores are equal to the mean of all item scores. Subscale scores can also be calculated in four areas: dissociative amnesia (items 2, absorp- tion and imaginative involvement (items 1, 7, 10, 18, 24, 28), depersonalization and derealization (items 3, 6, 9, 11, 13, 17, 20, 21, 25, 26, 29, 30), and passive influence (items 4, 14, 16, 19, 23). The Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) is a 28-item self-report measure (Bernstein Putnam, 1986). Scale items are presented as situations (e.g., "Some people have the experience of driving a car and suddenly realizing that they don remember what has happened during all or part of the trip" ). For each situation, subjects are asked to indi- cate how often these situations happen to them on a scale from 0 to 100, with 0 labeled "never" and 100 labeled "always." The DES measures experiences of gaps in memory and aware- ness, and experiences of derealization, depersonalization, absorption, and imaginative involvement. High test-retest reli- ability, internal reliability, and validity have been reported for the DES (Carlson Putnam, 1993). Procedure Instructions were read orally and students were assured that participation would be anonymous and voluntary. Subjects gave consent by their completion of the measures. Instructions for the completion of the measures were read by the students. The junior and senior high students com- pleted the A-DES first, followed by the demographic ques- tionnaire. The 14 to 15 year-old students completed the A- DES twice with a two week interval between testing. The college students completed the A-DES and the first, fol- lowed by the demographic questionnaire. The A-DES and the DES were presented in random order to test for order effects. Because the subject matter of the A-DES and the DES are sim- ilar, the first measure was completed and collected before 127 DISSOCIATION, Vol. lX, No. 2,, PI; "I I I 1 the second one was distributed to prevent subjects from com- paring their answers on the two measures. RESULTS Normative Data A one-way.ANOVA was performed onA-DES scores across age groups in the student samples. No significant differences were found. Means, standard deviations, and are reported in Table 1. A-DES subscale means across age groups are reported in Table 1. One-way ANOVAs were performed on A-DES scores across age groups in the student samples. significant dif- ferences were found. Reliability The 14 to 15 year-old subjects completed the A-DES on two occasions (two weeks apart) to assess for test-retest reli- ability. A Pearson product-moment correlation between scores on first and second administrations of the A-DES yield- ed a correlation coefficient (r = .77, < .00001, N = 22) for this analysis. As a measure internal consistency, Cronbach alphas were calculated for the global scale score as well as the four sub- scales. Alpha was .92 for the whole scale, .75 for the amne- sia subscale, .64 for the absorption subscale, .83 for deper- sonalization/derealization, and .77 for passive influence = 60 for all calculations). For all adolescent subjects, A-DES items were divided into two conceptually equivalent halves to assess internal consis- tency. A Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient between the two halves was calculated and corrected with the Spearman-Brown split-half reliability formula to yield an r for the whole scale of .94 (p < .00001, N = 60). Validity In the college sample, scores on the A-DES were corre- lated with scores on the DES, yielding an r of .77 (p < .00001, N = 46). There was no difference in A-DES scores for the dif- ferent test administration order (t = -1.79, df = 42, n.s.) DISCUSSION The results of this study provide support for the relia- bility and validity of the A-DES. The high split-half reliabili- ty and Cronbach alpha provide evidence that the measure has good internal consistency. Furthermore, high test-retest reliability indicates that the A-DES is able to measure con- sistently over time. Last, the strong relationship between A- DES and DES scores in the college population is evidence of the concurrent validity of the A-DES. Our study has limitations, however. The most notable limitationisthat we used a small, convenience sample of large- ly homogeneous school students. This non-random sample 128 greatly reduces the generalizahility of the study. Furthermore, conclusions about the construct validity of the scale would have been strengthened by examining A-DES scores of ado- lescents known to have high levels of dissociation. Without these data, the validity conclusions reached inthis study are preliminary. Further research should be conducted in order to gath- er more validity evidence for the A-DES. Comparing A-DES scores to other constructs thought to he related to dissocia- tion (e.g., fantasy proneness, openness to experience) would be valuable in assessing the validity of the measure. Outcome studies of adolescents in treatment for dissociative disorders would also help establish the construct validityof the A-DES. If the A-DES does prove to be reliable and valid upon further study, it may be useful for several purposes. Because the A-DES is designed to measure dissociation in several dif- ferent areas of normative dissociative phenomena such as dissociative amnesia, depersonalization, derealization, and absorption, researchers who wish to study normative pro- cesses in the adolescent population may be able to use the scale as a means of understanding various aspects of ado- lescent cognitive and emotional development and con- sciousness. Furthermore, the A-DES might be useful as a screening measure in some settings to identify those ado- lescents who are experiencing high levels of dissociation. This measure might aid clinicians by helping to quantify psy- chological symptoms that may be related to abuse or trau- ma histories. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association (1994). and statistical manual of mental disorders(4th ed) . Washington, DC: Author. Anastasi, A. (1988). New York: Macmillan. Armstrong, j., Putnam, F.W., Carlson, E.B., Libero, D., Smith, S.R. (1997). Development and validation of a measure of adoles- cent dissociation: The Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185,1-7. Bernstein, E.M. Putnam, F.W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174,727-734. Briere, J., Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested as children. 21-31. Briere, j., Runtz, M. (1988). Multivariate correlates of childhood psychological and physical maltreatment among university women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 12, Carlson, E.B., Armstrong, J. (1994). The diagnosis and assess- ment of dissociative disorders. In S.J. Lynn .W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives(pp. 159-174). New York: Guilford Press. DISSOCIATION, Vol. IX. No. 2. June 1996 SMITH/CARLSON Carlson, E.B., Armstrong, J., Loewenstein, R., Roth, D. (in press). Relationships between traumatic experiences and symptoms of post- traumatic stress, dissociation, and amnesia. In J.D. Bremner (Ed.). Trauma, memory, and dissociation. Washington, DC; American Psychiatric Press. Carlson, E.S., Putnam, F.W. (1993). An update on the dissocia- tive experiences scale. DISSOCIATION, 6, 16-27. Chu, J. A., Dill, D.L. (1990). Dissociative symptoms in relation to childhood physical and sexual abuse.American journal ofPsych.iatry, 147, 887-892. Classen, C., Koopman, C., Spiegel, D. (1993). Trauma and dis- sociation. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 57,178-194. Coons, P.M., Bowman, E., Fellow, T.A., Schneider, P. (1989) . Post- traumatic aspects of the treatment of victims of sexual abuse and incest.Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12,325-335. DiTomasso, M.J., Routh, D.K. (1993). Recall of abuse in child- hood and three measures of dissociation.ChildAbuse & Neglect, I77, 336-341. Goodwin, J.M., Cheeves, K., Connell, V. (1990). Borderline and other severe symptoms in adult survivors of incestuous abuse. Psychiatric Annals,20, 22-32. Kirby, J.S., Chu, J.A., Dill, D.L. (1993). Correlates of dissociative symptomatology in patients with physical and sexual abuse histo- ries. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34,258-263. Microsoft Word Version 6.0c computer software. (1994). The Microsoft Corporation. Nash, M.R., Hulsey, T.L., Sexton, M.C., Harralson, T.L., Lambert, W. (1993). Long-term sequelae of childhood sexual abuse: Perceived family environment, psychopathology, and dissociation.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61,276-283. Putnam, F.W. (1991). Dissociative disorders in children and ado- lescents: A developmental perspective. Psychiatric Clinicsof North America, 14,519-531. Putnam, F.W. (1.985). Dissociation as a response to extreme trau- ma. In R. Kluft (Ed.), Childhood antecedents of multiple personality(pp. 65-97).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Putnam, F.W. (1994). Dissociative disorders in children and ado- lescents. In SILynn IVY Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives 175-189). New York: Guilford Press. Putnam, F.W., Helmers, K., Trickett, P.K. (1993). Development, reliability, and validity of a child dissociation scale. Child Abuse& Neglect, 17, Sandberg, D.A., Lynn, S.J. (1992). Dissociative experiences, psy- chopathology and adjustment, and child and adolescent maltreat- ment in female college students.fournal ofAbnormalPsychology, 101, 717-723. Sanders, B., Giolas, M.H. (1991). Dissociation and childhood trau- ma in psychologically disturbed adolescents. American journal of Psychiatry, 148, 50-54. Swett, C., Halpert, M. (1993). Reported history of physical and sexual abuse in relation to dissociation and other symptornatolo- gy in women psychiatric inpatients.Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 545-555. van der Kolk, B.A. (1987). The psychological consequences nf over- whelming life experiences. In B. van der Kolk (Ed.), Psycholog ical trauma (pp. 1-30).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. van der Kolk, B.A., Kadish, W. (1987). Amnesia, dissociation, and the return of the repressed. In B. van der Kolk (Ed.), Psychological trauma (pp. 173-190) . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 129 DISSOCIATION, Vol. IX, No. ?.June 1996