PSYCHOGENIC AMNESIA: A CLINICAL INVESTIGATION OF 25 CASES Philip M. Coons, I.D. Victor Milstein, Ph.D. Philip M. Coons, M.D., is Associate Professor of Psychiatry at Indiana University School of Medicine and Staff Psychiatrist at Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital in Indianapolis, Indiana. Victor Milstein, Ph.D., is Professor of Psychiatry at Indiana University School of Medicine and Psychophysiologist at Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital in Indianapolis, Indiana. For reprints write Philip M. Coons, M.D., Larue D. Carter Memorial Hospital, 1315 West 10th Street, Indianapolis, Indiana 46202. Portions of this paperwere presented at the Sixth International Conference on Multiple Personality/Dissociative States, October 13, 1989, Chicago, Illinois. ABSTRACT Although there have been eight descriptive studies on patients with psychogenic amnesia between 1935 and 1962, this is the first such study in thirty years. Using an extensive clinical history, mental sta- tus examination, collateral interviews, neurological examination, electroencephalogram (EEG), intelligence testing, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES), the authors examined twenty-five consec- utive psychogenic amnesia patients diagnosed with modified DSM- 11I-R not of sudden onset. A few cases were repetitive. There were multi- ple stressful psychological precipitants. There were many similari- ties to patients with multiple personality disorder, providing further proof that dissociative disorders occur along a dissociative spectrum. Psychogenic amnesia is probably the most common dissociative dis- order diagnosis. It is frequently overlooked because the symptoms are subtle, and other primary diagnoses are often present. INTRODUCTION With the exception of several case reports (Daniel Crovitz, 1986; Feldman, 1967; MacHovec, 1981) and an excel- lent review (Loewenstein, 1991), very little has appeared in the literature on psychogenic amnesia since the early 1960s. At least six recent case reports have confused psychogenic amnesia and fugue (Akhtar, Lindsey, Khan, 1981; Gudjonsson Haward, 1982; Lyon, 1985; Kaszniak, Berren, Santiago, 1988; Takahashi, 1988; Eisen, 1989). Since all of the individuals described in these case reports were dis- covered far from their usual place of work or residence, they really met the (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) , and DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) , cri- teria for psychogenic fugue, atypical dissociative disorder, or dissociative disorder not otherwise specified (DDNOS). Although psychogenic amnesia did not become an offi- cial diagnosis until the publication of DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), it was listed as a symptom under dissociative reaction in theDSM-Iand under hysterical neu- rosis, dissociative type, in the (American Psychiatric Association, 1968). Between 1935 and 1962, there appeared eight descrip- tive studies on large series of patients with amnesia (Abeles Schilder, 1935; Leavitt, 1935; Kanzer, 1939-1940; Sargant Slater, 1941; Parfitt Gall, 1944; Wilson Rupp, 1950; Kennedy Neville, 1957; Kiersch, 1962). Numbers of patients ranged from 30 to 144, Diagnoses for these amnes- tic patients included what is now known as psychogenic sia (American Psychiatric Association, 1987), plus sizeable numbers of patients with psychogenic fugue, epilepsy, vari- ous types of organic brain disorders, and malingering. Selected findings from studies are summarized in Table 1. These earlier studies, excluding those done with armed forces subjects, showed an approximately equal sex incidence. Although psychogenic amnesia could occur at almost any age (range 13 - 66) , the peak incidence of cases occurred in the third and fourth decades. Although most studies did not comment on the possibility of recurrence, 15 of 63 (24%) patients in the Abeles Schilder study (1935) had two or more episodes. The only studieswhich mentioned incidence reported widely varying figures (range .26 to 14.4%). The length of an episode lasted anywhere from a few hours to three weeks or longer, but the majority lasted from just under twenty-four hours (43%) to between one and five days (33%) , and 75% recovered spontaneously (Abeles Schilder, 1935) . In the majority of studies, malingering was felt to have been present in some patients. In two more recent studies (MacHovec, 1981; Schachter, Wang, Tulving, Freedman, 1982; and Kopelman, 1987), memory loss was found to be autobiographical in nature. The present study is part of a larger investigation of dis- sociative disorders including multiple personality disorder (MPD), psychogenic fugue, and DDNOS (Coons, Bowman, Milstein, 1988) conducted over a five-year period from 1984 to 1989. The patients reported herein were the first 25 consecutive psychogenic amnesia patients who presented to a dissociative disorders clinic in a tertiary care hospital and outpatient clinic which was affiliated with a major medical 73 DISSOCIATION, Vol. V, No. 2, June 1992 PSYCHOGENIC AMNESIA TABLE 1 Previous Studies of Psychogenic Amnesia Study (Year) Number of Source of Decade of Peak Gender Ratio Precipitating Patients Patients Incidence Incidence MIF Stresses Abeles & Schilder (1935) 63 Inpatient Psychiatric 3 .26 31132 marital, financial Leavitt (1935) 104 General Hospital .83 66138 emotional shock Kamer (1939-40) 71 Inpatient Psychiatric 3 41130 marital, financial Sargant & Slater (1941) 144 Inpatient Neurology 14.4 combat Partin & Gall (1944) 30 Royal Air Force 3-4 NA discipline Wilson & Rupp (1950) 59 General Hospital 3-4 31128 marital Kennedy & Neville (1957) 74 Emergency Room 52122 marital, financial Kiersch (1962) 98 Army Gen Hosp 3 9216 legal Contributing Factors conversion, head injury, epilepsy psychosis, epilepsy depression, alcohol, conversion, epilepsy depression, head injury, psychosis conversion, alcohol epilepsy, head injury, psychosis epilepsy, head injury, psychosis alcohol, head injury center and school. The purpose of the study was to describe and characterize patients with psychogenic amnesia and com- pare them to patients with multiple personality disorder. SUBJECTS AND METHODS Subjects The subjects in this study were the first 25 consecutive patients with amnesia who presented to the dissociative dis- orders clinic for treatment or consultation. There were 17 inpatients and eight outpatients. All but seven were patients at our facility. All patients who were administered experi- mental assessment instruments gave their voluntary informed consent. Methods Psychogenic amnesia was defined according to DSM-IH and DSM-III-R criteria with the exception that the amnesic episode was not required to be of "sudden" and presumably recent onset. This particular convention was adopted because of the relatively large numbers of patients presenting to the dissociative disorders clinic with chronic amnesia for large portions of childhood. On intake patients were assessed by a comprehensive history including psychiatric, medical, family, and social por- tions in addition to a mental status examination. Previous psychiatric records were requested and the referring clini- cian was consulted. In most cases a collateral history was taken from a family member. Because previous reports of psychogenic amnesia fre- quently mentioned the presence of organic factors such as head injury, the patients were given a complete physical exam- ination including a neurological examination and EEG. Intelligence was measured by use of the Shipley-Hartford Vocabulary Test (Zachary, 1986) or WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981). The MMPI (Hathaway McKinley, 1967) was admin- istered to measure personality functioning. Other rating instru- ments including the Global Assessment Scale or GAS (Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss,1976) , Hamilton Depression Scale or Ham-D (Hamilton, 1967) , and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale or BPRS (Overall Gorham, 1962). When the Dissociative Experiences Scale or DES (Bernstein Putnam, 1986) was developed, it was added to the assessment pack- age. The data were analyzed using t-tests where continuous variables were present and chi square for categorical vari- ables unless cell frequencies were less than 5 when the Fisher Exact Test was used. A probability level of .05 wasused as determining significantdifferences. RESULTS Demographic Characteristics The patients included 23 women and two men ranging in age from 17 to 51 years (mean, 32 years). Only seven (28%) were currently married. The remainder were either single (40%) or separated or divorced (32%). Mean edu- cational level was 12.7 years (range, 9-18 years). All were Caucasian except for one Hispanic woman. The majority (64%) were employed. One was a student, and two were homemakers. 74 DiSSOCI.VrlON, Vol. V, No. 2. fune l992 COONS/MILSTEIN Onset of Illness These patients reported that their amnesia began between the ages of 2 and 37 (mean, 19 years). Five were unable to give a specific age of onset, other than early child- hood. Seventeen patients required psychiatric hospitaliza- tion and their first hospitalization occurred at a mean age of 27 years (range, 14-41 years) . For all 25 patients, the mean age of first contact with mental health professionals was also 27 years (range, 5-57 years). Presenting Psychiatric Symptoms These patients presented with a wide variety of symp- toms (Table 2), many of which were interrelated. Although all of the patients had amnesia, few actually complained of memory loss. Depression was the most common symptom. Conversion symptoms were infrequent; there were two cases of anaesthesia and one case each of blindness, deafness, apho- nia, and paralysis. A minority of the patients (36%) had a history of legal difficulties. Two patients were involved with each of the following offenses: driving while intoxicated, prostitution, and writing bad checks. One patient was involved with theft, one with embezzlement, one with sell- ing drugs, and one with child abuse. The most common type of sexual dysfunction was inhibited sexual desire, found in 40%. Other somatic complaints, which ranged from 20 to 37%, included nausea, abdominal pain, dysmenorrhea, pal- pitations, dizziness, and dyspnea. While headaches were the third most frequent complaint, they tended to be of the ten- sion variety (N=10), although four patients had migraine headaches and two had both varieties. Occurrence of other dissociative symptoms characteristic ofMPD, such as audi- tory hallucinations, fugues, or alter ego states, were notably absent or greatly diminished. The most common presenting personality traits and symp- tomswere borderline, dependent, and histrionic. These includ- ed the following: affective instability (84%), dependency or helplessness(64%v), unstable, intense interpersonal rela- tionships (60%) , physically self-damaging acts (60%) , impul- sivity or unpredictability (56%) , identity disturbance (48%) , inability to work consistently (48%), self-dramatization (44%), school grades below expected IQ level (44%), irra- tional angry outbursts (40%), shallow or lacking genuine- TABLE 2 Presenting Symptoms of Psychogenic Amnesia Symptom Psychogenic Amnesia Multiple Personality p Value(n=25) (N=50) N(%) N(%) Amnesia 25 (100) 50 (100) ns Depression 21 (84) 44 (88) ns Headaches 16 (64) 18 (56) ns Sexual Dysfunction 15 (60) 42 (84) .05 Somatization 11 (44) 18 (36) ns Depersonalization 10 (40) 19 (38) ns Auditory Hallucinations 6 (24) 36 (72) .005 Alcohol Abuse 6 (24) 21 (42) ns Conversion 6 (24) 20 (40) ns Drug Abuse 5 (20) 23 (46) .05 Self-Mutilation 5 (20) 17 (34) .005 Binge Eating 5 (20) 1 (2) ns Delusions 3 (1 2) 3 (6) ns Visual Hallucinations 3 (12) 8 (16) ns Fugue 1 (4) 24 (48) .005 Anorexia 1 (4 ) 2 (4) ns Pseudoseizures 0 7 (14) ns 75 DISSOC1ATt0N. Vol. V. No. 2, June 1992 PSYCHOGENIC AMNESIA ness (40%) , inability to relax (40%) , and intolerance of being alone (40%). Previous The mean number of previous psychiatric diagnoses was 2.6 (range, 0-8) . The most common diagnoses included major depression (60%), substance abuse or dependence (40%), eating disorder (28%), borderline personality disorder (24%), alcohol abuse or dependence (24%), dysthymia (20%), adjustment disorder (12%), generalized anxiety disorder (12%), and conversion disorder (8%). The patients aver- aged four years between their first mental health contact and being diagnosed with psychogenic amnesia. Amnesia The character of the amnesia was quite variable. Few actually complained of memory loss. Four (46%) had amne- sia for recent events, 15 (60%), had amnesia for remote events, and six (24%) had both recent and remote memo- ry loss. The majority (60%) had only one episode of amne- sia, but eight (32%) had two episodes, and two (8%) had four episodes. The duration of an episode of recent amne- sia ranged from 15 minutes to four hours (mean, 1.2 hours) . The duration of a remote episode of amnesia ranged from one month to 20 years (mean, 7.7 years). Typical descrip- tions of amnesia would include, "I can remember anything before the age of 12, " or "I can t remember the third and fourth grades." Most patients (76%) experienced the selec- tive type of amnesia, but two reported the generalized type, and four (16%) experienced both types. No cases of con- tinuous or generalized amnesia were observed. but also involved mothers, uncles, siblings, and neighbors. Family History of PsychiatricDisorder Alcohol abuse or dependency was the most common psychiatric illness reported in first degree relatives. It was reported in 16% of fathers, 12% of mothers, and 8% of sib- lings. Major depression was present equally in mothers and siblings (12%), while drug abuse or dependence was report- ed four times as frequently in siblings as compared to moth- ers (16% vs. 4%) . Personality disorders were reported more often in fathers (8%) than in mothers or siblings (4% each). No dissociative disorders were reported in first degree rela- tives. Medical and Neurological Problems Obesity was the most common Axis III diagnosis, fol- lowed by migraine headaches and spastic colitis (16% each). Only one patient showed neurological findings, an unre- lated Bell palsy. None had seizure histories, and only three had positive EEG findings. These included occasional gen- eralized spike and wave discharges, diffuse sharp and slow wave discharges, and bilateral temporal lobe spikes, respec- tively. Psychological Testing The MMPI profile and mean T scores for the 19 female patients with psychogenic amnesia are shown in Figure 1. These scores are contrasted with scores from 38 females with MPD. The curves for both groups are quite similar, and both show a number of scales with T scores of 70 or greater. For both groups, the highest score occurred on the schizophre- nia scale, but depression, psychopathic deviance, paranoia, Precipitants of Amnesia The previous literature on psy- chogenic amnesia was examined for dif- ferent types of precipitants to an amnesic episode (See Table 1) . Nine categories were selected, and the patients were rated on whether or not a specific precipitant was present. The results are displayed in Table 3. The psychogenic amnesia may have had more than one precipi- tant. Although child abuse occurred most frequently, disavowed behaviors (sexu- al promiscuity, suicide attempt, self- mutilation, crime, or running away) were present in many others. Thus, multiple interrelational as well as dependent precipitants of psychogenic amnesia are suggested by the frequency and vari- ety of factors reported by the patients. Childhood trauma was very common in this group of patients. Sexual abuse was reported by 52% and physical abuse by 40%. Neglect (16%) and abandon- ment (12%) were also present. Only 28% reported no childhood trauma. Perpetrators of child abuse were usual- ly fathers (36%) or stepfathers (12%), TABLE 3 Possible Precipitants of Amnesia of Psychogenic Amnesia Precipitant N (%v ) Child Abuse 15 (60) Marital Trouble 6 (24) Disavowed Sexual Behavior 4 (16) Suicide Attempt 4 (16) Criminal Behavior 3 (12) Death of Relative 1 (4) Psychotherapy 1 (4) Runaway 1 (4) Unknown 4 (16) N is greater than 25 because of multiple precipitants present in each case. 76 DISSOCGITLON, I'M. V. No. 2, ,lime 1992 COONS/MILSTEIN (mean = 61.4) manifested only some mild symptoms (t = 7.206, p < .0001) . The DES scores from the 22 psy- chogenic amnesia patients yielded a mean of 39.5 (range, 26-56), sug- gesting somewhat less dissociation than would be expected from a group of patients with MPD. DSM-111-R Diagnoses Psychogenic amnesia was a prin- cipal diagnosis in only five patients. It was a secondary or tertiary diag- nosis in all of the other patients. Overall, there were an additional 60 DSM-III-R Axis I or Axis II diagnoses (mean, 2.4; range 1-5). Personality disorders were the most common Axis II diagnoses (68%). In eight indi- viduals (32%), personality disorder symptomatology was mixed, and six (24%) met criteriafor borderline per- sonality disorder. Affective disorders were the most common Axis I diag- nosis (56%). Seven (28%) had dys- thymia, three (21%) had major depression, and one (4%) had adjust- ment disorder with depression. Alcohol abuse/dependence was pre- sent in six (24%) and drug abuse/dependence in five (20%). Six (24%) had conversion disorder, and five (20%) had bulimia. Two patients had schizophrenia, and one had a brief psychosis. FIGURE 1 MMPI in Psychogenic Amnesia and Multiple Personality 1 / 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 l 1 1r 7 s -0 - - - -PSYCHOGENIC AMNESIA(19 FEMALES) ___ t __. MULTIPLE PERSONALITY (38 FEMALES) L F K Hs D Hy Pd M t Pa PI Sc M a Si Nw cc0 0N I- and psychasthenia were also high. The F validity scale was also quite high for both groups and, combined with the rel- atively low K score, suggest that all of these patients were open to admissions of pathology. This does not necessarily imply faking bad. Intelligence was in the average range for the 10 patients who were tested with the WAIS-R, with a mean Verbal IQ of 107, mean Performance IQ of 102, and mean Full Scale IQ of 102. The Shipley-Hartford Vocabulary Test yielded a mean IQof 122 for another eight patients. Standardized Measures of Psychopathology On the Ham-D(possible scores 24-97), the 17 inpatients had a mean score of 46.9 (SD, 10.5), which, while suggest- ing a moderate degree of depression, was significantly high- er (t = 2.449, p < .02), than the score for the eight outpa- tients with MPD (mean = 39.9, SD = 9.8). Both inpatient and outpatient groups showed about the same level of psy- chopathology on the BPRS, but there was a significant dif- ference in GAS scores with the psychogenic amnesia inpa- tients, indicating serious symptomatology or impairment (mean = 42.8), while the psychogenic amnesia outpatients DISCUSSION Although there are many similarities between patients with psychogenic amnesia in this study and those in previ- ous studies, there are also many differences. As in previous studies (Abeles Schilder, 1935; Leavitt, 1935; Kanzer, 1939- 1940; Parfitt Gall, 1944) , the peak incidence of psychogenic amnesia in our study occurs in the third and fourth decades. Similarly, we found a significant incidence of somatic com- plaints, including headaches (Kanzer, 1939-1940; Parfitt Gall, 1944) and conversion (Abeles Schilder, 1935), depression (Kanzer, 1939-1940; Sargant Slater, 1941), and alcohol abuse (Kanzer, 1939-1940; Parfitt Gall, 1944). We also found a small number of patients who experienced psy- chotic symptoms (Leavitt, 1935; Sargant Slater, 1941; Wilson Rupp, 1950; Kennedy Neville, 1957). However, unlike previous earlier studies, we found a much higher incidence of chronicity. Our patients were nei- ther acute nor did their amnesia resolve spontaneously. Our 77 DISSOCIATION, Vol. V, No. 2, June 1992 PSYCHOGENIC AMNESIA sample may be biased because our hospital is tertiary-care, and acute patients are selected out by their rapid recovery. Likewise, acute financial or marital crises tend to be resolved prior to admission to a tertiary-care facility. Since we were not engaged in a war at the time of the study and our facil- ity is not a Veterans Administration facility, there were no combat-related cases of psychogenic amnesia. TheDSM-III- R criteria for psychogenic amnesia effectively excluded any patients with amnesia secondary to organic causes such as head injury, alcoholism, etc. The extremely high incidence of psychogenic amnesia in women in this study (92%) was an unexpected finding since previous studies, except those includingveterans, found an approximately equal sex distribution. The reasons for the high proportion of women in this study is speculative, but maybe because women are more often sexually abused than men in childhood. Or, it may be because men are under- represented among those seeking psychiatric treatment in general. Another unexpected finding was that 40% of our sam- ple had two or more episodes of psychogenic amnesia. Abeles Schilder (1935) reported a 24% incidence of recurrence. The higher rate of recurrence may be explained by the increased chronicity of patients found in tertiary-care set- tings. The incidence of psychogenic amnesia among our adult service patients is 1.8%; this is between the figures report- ed in the literature: from the low range ofAbeles and Schilder (1935) and Leavitt (1935) for patients admitted to psychi- atric units (.26 and .83, respectively) to the 14.4% rate report- ed by Sargant Slater (1941) for patients admitted to neu- rological units. One might expect the incidence of amnesic syndromes to be higher on neurological units because mem- ory dysfunction is a common neurological complaint. Unlike previous studies (Kanzer, 1939-1940; Wilson Rupp, 1950; Kennedy Neville, 1957; Kirsch, 1962), we found no patients whom we thought were malingering. Most patients with severe legal problems who might have had rea- son to malinger were selected out of our sample by admis- sion criteria prohibiting most admissions of the criminally disordered offender to our hospital. However, a significant proportion of our patients (30%) had amnesia based on dis- avowed behaviors such as sexual behavior, suicide attempts, and running away, and may have had some reason to at least minimize their behavior. The diagnosis of malingering is difficult (Resnick, 1984), and has only recently been added to the diagnostic criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) . The similarities between patients with psychogenic amnesia in this study and patients with multiple personali- ty disorder in other studies is striking (Bliss, 1984; Putnam, Guroff, Silberman, Barban Post, 1986; Coons, Bowman, Milstein, 1988). Like patients with MPD, there is a high incidence of amnesia, depression, headaches, somatization, conversion, and depersonalization. However, in this study (See Table 2), there was a significantly lower incidence of auditory hallucinations, sexual dysfunction, substance abuse, conversion, self-mutilation, and fugue. In our studies of patients with MPD and psychogenic amnesia, the occurrence of abuse was more frequent in patients withMPD (p < .01) and was more apt to be repeated (p < .05) . Comparing the mean MMPI profiles of women with psychogenic amnesia in this study to the mean MMPI profiles of women MPD patients in our previous study (Coons, Bowman, Milstein, 1988) , reveals striking similarities (See Figure 1). Only the F and Sc scales were lower (p < .1) in the psychogenic amnesia group. These findings lend credence to the theory that dissociative dis- orders occur along a dissociative spectrum (Braun, 1988). CONCLUSIONS The findings in this study have implications for Since psychogenic amnesia may be chronic (i.e., large blocks of amnesia may be present in the past lives of individuals), the term "sudden" should be removed from criterion A of the diagnosis. The chronic form of psychogenic amnesia appears to be more common than previously realized. The more acute forms of psychogenic amnesia are probably quite unusual, and this may explain why they achieve newspaper and television notoriety when they do occur. It is difficult to diagnose psychogenic amnesia because patients rarely complain of amnesia. Thus, it will only be reliably diagnosed when clinicians become accustomed to taking detailed chronological life histories and making detailed inquiries about areas of memory loss. Merely ask- ing patients about their knowledge of themselves, such as if they graduated from high school or what their grades were, will fail to elicit evidence of psychogenic amnesia (Loewenstein, 1991). Although psychogenic amnesia usually consists of a sin- gle episode, it may recur on more than one occasion. In addition, dissociation exists on a spectrum from normal to increasingly abnormal. Psychogenic amnesia is perhaps the simplest form of dissociation since, unlike the other disso- ciative disorders, amnesia is present with neither identity change nor wandering. Modern research about the phenomenology and etiol- ogy of psychogenic amnesia is in its infancy much like research of 15 years ago. We now possess powerful tools such as the DES (Bernstein Putnam, 1986) and the SCID-D (Steinberg, Rounsaville, Cicchetti, 1990) to study disso- ciation, and future studies should make use of these tools. Hopefully, the future will bring a renaissance in the study of all dissociative disorders, not just Studies on at least two of the dissociative disorders, depersonalization disor- der, and psychogenic fugue, will need to utilize a multicen- ter approach because of the rarity of these disorders. REFERENCES Abeles, M., Schilder, P. (1935). Psychogenic loss of personal identity: Amnesia. 34, 587-604. Akhtar, S., Lindsey,B., Khan, F.L. (1981). Sudden amnesia for personal identity. 84, 46-48. 78 DISSOCIATION, Vol. V. No. 1 June 1992 COONS/MILSTEIN American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Manual of Mental Disorders (p. 32). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Manual of Mental Disorders (2nd Edition, p. 40). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd Edition, p. 253-260). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd Edition, revised, pp. 269-277). Washington, DC: Author. Bernstein, E.M., Putnam, F.W. (1986). Development, reliabili- ty, and validity of a dissociation scale. Disease,174, 727-735. Bliss, E.L. (1984). A symptom profile of patients with multiple per- sonality disorder, including MMPI results. Mental Disease,172, 197-202. Braun, B.G. (1988). The BASK (behavior, affect, sensation, knowl- edge) model of dissociation.DISSOCIATION,I (1), 4-23. Coons, P.M., Bowman, E.S., Milstein, V. (1988). Multiple per- sonality disorder: A clinical investigation of 50 cases. and Mental Disease,176, 519-527. Coons, P.M., Milstein, V. (1986). Psychosexual disturbances in multiple personality: Characteristics, etiology, and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,47, 106-110. Daniel, W.F., Crovitz, H.F. (1986) . ECT-induced alteration of psy- chogenic amnesia. 74, 302-303. Eisen, M.R. (1989). Return of the repressed: Hypnoanalysis of a case of total amnesia. Hypnosis, 37, 107-119. Endicott, J., Spitzer, R.L., Fleiss, J. (1976) . The GAS. Psychiatry, 33,766-771. Feldman, F. (1967). A clinical note: The use of oral tranquilizers to relieve amnesia.International Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 3, 138- 139. Gudjonsson, G.H., Haward, L.R.C. (1982). Case report - hys- terical amnesia as an alternative to suicide. Law,22, 68-72. Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 278-296. Hathaway, S.R., McKinley,J.C. (1967).MMPIManuaL New York: Psychological Corporation. Kanzer, M.N. (1939-1940). Amnesia: A statistical study. Journal of Psychiatry, 96,711-766. Kaszniak, A.W., Berren, M.R., Santiago, J. (1988). Amnesia as a consequence of male rape: A case report 97, 100-104. Kennedy, A., Neville, J. (1957). Sudden loss of memory. Medical Journal 2, 428-433. Kiersch, T.A. (1962) . Amnesia: A clinical study of ninety-eight cases. American Journal of Psychiatry,199, 57-60. Kopelman, M.D. (1987). Amnesia: Organic and psychogenic. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, Leavitt, F.H. (1935) . Etiology of temporary amnesia. of Psychiatry, 91,1079-1088. Loewenstein, R.J. (1991). Psychogenic amnesia and psychogenic fugue: A comprehensive review. In A. Tasman S.M. Goldfinger, American Psychiatric Press Review of Psychiatry, 10, pp. 189-221). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Lyon, L.S. (1985). Facilitating telephone number recall in a case of psychogenic amnesia. Experimental Psychiatry, MacHovec, FT (1981). Hypnosis to facilitate recall in psychogenic amnesia and fugue states: Treatment variables. Clinical Hypnosis,24, 7-13. Overall, J.E., Gorham, D.R. (1962). The brief psychiatric rating scale. Reports,10, 799-812. Parfitt,D.N., Gall, C.M.C. (1944). Psychogenic amnesia: The refusal to remember. 90, 511-531. Putnam, F.W. (1989). Pierre Janet and modern views of dissocia- tion.Journal of Traumatic Stress,2, 413-429. Putnam, F.W., Guroff, J J., Silberman, E.K, Barban, L., Post, R.M. (1986) . The clinical phenomenologyofMPD: A review of 100 recent cases. 285-293. Resnick, P j. (1984). The detection of malingered illness. Sciences and the Law, Sargent, W., Slater, E. (1941) . Amnesic syndromes in war. of the Royal Society of Medicine, Schacter, D.L., Wang, P.L., Tulving, E., Freedman, M. (1982). Functional retrograde amnesia: A quantitative case study. Neuropsychologia, 523-532. Steinberg, M., Rounsaville, B., Cicchetti, D.V. (1990). The struc- tured clinical interviewforDSM-III-Rdissociative disorders: Preliminary report on a new diagnostic instrument. 149, 76-82. Takahashi, Y. (1988). Aokigahara jukai: Suicide and amnesia in Mt. Fugi Black Forest. 18, 164- 174. Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-Revised. Corporation. Wilson, G., Rupp, C. (1950). Amnesia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 106,481-485. Zachary, R.A. (1986). Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. 79 DISSOCLATION. Vol. V, No. 2, June 1992