NORMAL AND PATHOLOGICAL DISSOCIATIONS OF EARLY CHILDHOOD Theresa K . Albini, M.S.W., A.C.S.W., L.C.S.W. Terri E . Pease . Ph.D. (cand .) Theresa K . Albini, M .S.W., A .C .S.W., L .C.S.W. is in private practice in Chicago, Illinois . Terri E . Pease, Ph .D. (cand .) is a doctoral candidate in child and family psychopathology at Cornell University and a professor in the Division of Psychol- ogy and Counseling at Governors State University, Univer- sity Park, Illinois. For reprints write : Theresa K . Albini, M .S.W., L.C .S.W., 300 N . State Street, Suite 3707, Chicago, Illinois 60610. An earlier version of this paper was presented to the Second International Conference on Multiple Personality/Disso- ciative States, Chicago, Illinois, 1985. This work was completed in part when Ms . Albini was affiliated with The Institute of Juvenile Research and the Department of Child Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago. ABSTRACT The authors hypothesize that multiple personality disorder is related to the processes that lead to the formation in children of a distinct and cohesive self. Three clinical propositions concerning MPD derived from this hypothesis are : first, multiple personality disorder should be seen as a childhood disorder, second, cohesion of the self is best understood as a developmental achievement mediated by specific experiences in the early years of ; third, some dissociative disorders, including multiple personality disorder, are survivals of an earlier personality organization in which distinct centers of experience and initiative existed within a single individual. NORMAL AND PATHOLOGICAL DISSOCIATIONS OF EARLY CHILDHOOD The problem of multiple personality disorder (MPD) has generated much controversy and enthusiasm in recent years . This phenomenon of distinct identities possessing most of the behavioral and psychological characteristics of "separate persons " in a single body is a fascinating one. Those affected exhibit discontinuous behavior, emotion, and identity, features which are often part of the typical developmental presentation of young children . It is not known if all children have dissociative-like states . Dissocia- tive potential has been cited as being biologically deter- mined (Braun & Sachs 1985, p . 43) . The following is a developmental focus upon premorbid factors which may 144 correlate with dissociation . Comparisons of normal children with very young victims of severe abuse lead us to suspect that the momentary establishment of centers of experience ex- ternal to the core self during transient hypnotic-like states may be typical in young children . Such episodes may com- prise a normal developmental analogue to the dissociative phenomena characteristic of MPD, such as described by Kluft (1984a) as Factor I (in the Four Factor Theory of the etiology of MPD), the capacity to dissociate . The theoretical underpinnings of the ideas about childhood development presented in this paper rely heavily on some major tenets of the psychology of the self as formulated by Heinz Kohut (1971, 1977, 1984) and expanded upon by his followers. Although Kohut 's salient concepts of the "cohesive self' and the "vertical split " (disavowal) have been described by previ- ous authors who have contributed to the MPD literature (Berman, 1981 ; Gruenewald, 1977 ; Greaves, 1980), their theses predated the later expansion of his findings and his models . By 1977 Kohut's theory of the development of the self encircled and transcended Freud's instinct theory. Narcissism, instead of being relegated to a stage of early development, was described as having its own developmen- tal line, paralleling the developmental line of object rela- tions . Thus, pregenital as well as Oedipal pathology could be understood as disorders of the self (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). Therefore, a brief overview and definitions of the key terms utilized in this paper will be presented in order to provide a frame of reference. Kohut describes " . . . the growth of the self experience as a physical and mental unit which has cohesiveness in space and continuity in time " (1971, p . 118) . Elsewhere he ob- serves: . . . the developmental path of the experience of his (i .e ., the child's) self is separate from that followed by his ex- perience of the single body parts and single bodily and mental functions . . . .the child's experience of his body parts and of their functions and of his various mental activities has its own line of development ; that this devel- opment leads toward the increasing neutralization of these experiences, toward the increasing recognition of the spatial interrelatedness of various body parts and of the cooperation of their various functions . (Kohut, 1978, pp . 748-749) Kohut (1978) states that the stage of the cohesive self " . . . begins at the point when the (separately developing) self has DISSOCIATION . Vol. 11, \o :September 1989 +1 I become strong enough to gain ascendency over the experi- ential world of the body-mind parts" (pp, 747-48, f .n . 3). The above is especially relevant when we consider the infant and/or toddler 's subjective experiences of any inap- propriate or brutal physical intrusions by their caretakers. Muslin (1981) states: . . . the self refers to a specific content of mental appar- atus, a cohesive configuration of the mind which con- tains the collection of percepts of one's body and mind . It is most importantly understood as the individ- ual's experience of a unified assortment of features of his body and mind at any one time . While there are several selves, there is only one that is experienced as basic and resistant to change . Kohut terms it the `nuclear self .' This self contains the central, self- assertive goals and purposes of the individual, as well as his talents and skills (p . 9). Kohut (1971, 1977} states that barring inherited factors, the nascent self comes into being with the capacity for achieving cohesion . He refers to self-object functions as the "psychological oxygen" needed to realize this capacity, just as well formed lungs require oxygen to achieve their life sustaining capacity . The major danger to the vulnerable self is disintegration (loss of cohesion), signaled by disintegra- tion anxiety which includes fragmentation, serious enfee- blement, or uncontrollable rage . Kohut (1977) describes fragmentation as " . . . the (child's) estrangement from his body and mind in space, the breakup of the sense of his continuity in time" (p .105) . Self-object functions are divided into three basic forms : mirroring, twinship (a specific kind of mirroring) and idealizing . Mirroring consists of empathic admiration, approval, echoing the unfolding self, and thus offering the child in developmentally age-appropriate and phase-specific ways confirmation of the child's overall worth, and appreciation of his talents and skills . Twinship consists of the confirmation of the self s need to experience the presence of essential alikeness with others . The idealizing self-object functions are those of calming, soothing, tension regulation, protection and guidance . Optimal self-objects are empathically attuned with the subtle and gradually changing self-object needs of the self moving along its developmental lines . Since the infant and preoedipal child does not really experience these functions .as distinct from his own self, he does not perceive the provider of the functions as a discrete object but as a so called "self-object ." There is an infinite diversity of self-object experiences from all phases of the life cycle . Kohut (1984) proposes we never outgrow our need for self-objects ; rather the self-object functions are gradually transformed from archaic to mature forms through the process of transmuting internalizations. Kohut and Wolf (1978) state that the nuclear self crystallizes as a result of this process of intrapsychic structuralization. Gedo and Goldberg (1973) state: It should be emphasized that cognitive differentiation between the self and an object in the external world is achieved much earlier, usually before the end of the first year of life ; . . .Long after the achievement of this cognitive distinction, the child continues to utilize the object as part of his narcissistic world (p . 61). Tolpin (1978) observes that " . . . structural deficits occur when the child's self-objects fail to meet normal endowment half-way and do not provide the indispensable transitional precursors of psychic structures which gradually undergo internalization and effectively maintain the vitality, initia- tive, and self-esteem of a cohesive self ' (p .172) . Muslin and Val (1987) describe self/self-objects by saying, "These early relationships are experienced as fusions or mergers, i .e .., immersions (psychologically speaking) into the body and mind of the caretaking self-object" (p .27). Tolpin (1978) points out how " . . . Mahler et al .'s (1975) theory of a decisive, pathogenic 'rapproachment conflict' . . and Kernberg's (1975) theory of pathological idealizes tion, . . . grandiosity, and archaic conflicts misunderstand transference revivals of legitimate developmental needs toward their legitimate self-objects" (p .180) . She goes on to say: This "change of emphasis, " and the shift in point of view from conflict to structural development have been made possible by the discovery of the missing piece of the childhood psychic reality of an expectable environment and its indispensable psychological functions - a prestructural self-object "environment" which for all practical purposes is indistinguishable from the child's own mental organization and his cohesiveness or lack thereof . The concept of self-object as the precursor of psychic structure is the indispensable theoretical bridge which now links the most important contributions of psychoanalytic developmental psychology to a theory which is consistent with the child's need for structure, his normal and abnormal structural development, and to a theory of analytic treatment which actually fosters a needed process of further structural growth . (Tolpin, 1978, p . 181) We propose that in traumatized young children, prior to the formation of a nuclear self, whose environments provide inadequate soothing (where Kluft's [1984a] Factors II, overwhelming stimuli, and IV, inadequate stimulus barriers, are present) a few self nuclei vertically split off from (dis- avowed) the core self may be used and reused, because their capacity to support self soothing is superior to any other available to the child and is the child's only alternative to its traumatic true environment, (Kluft's [1984a] Factor III, shaping influences) . It may be that it is not solely the capacity to form these external condensation points that distin- guishes MPD children from normals, but the persistence of such external nuclei and the firmness of the vertical-split barrier that separates them from the center nucleus and prevents their becoming integrated into the core of the child's self . Thus, varied aspects of the self co-exist without integration. Two cases of probable MPD-like phenomena in very young children in treatment are presented, and contrasted 145 DISSOCIATION, Vol . 3: September 1989 with similar but non-trauma-based self soothing dissocia- tions in children of similar ages. CASE ILLUSTRATION ONE The child, Pam, was a 4 year old girl whom the therapist had seen in weekly psychotherapy since the age of 2 years and 9 months when she was first referred for evaluation of her developmental status . She told the therapist about a monster to whom she gave the name of a popular TV figure, who comes into her room and scares her and bites her . This child had first been referred for multiple fractures on x-ray before 6 months of age . There were also several opportuni- ties for retraumatization of this child . In the course of her treatment there was working through of much of her fear of injury, her tendency to be afraid to he alone, and her difficulties with repeated traumas related to indecision on the part of systems involved in deciding her ultimate cus- tody . There had been one early hint of distinct selves crystal- lizing in this child . In an earlier session she had emphatically denied (while cutting up a lump of play dough into several identical-sized pieces - an important activity) that she was not Pam but was Karen, giving the name of a baby of her acquaintance. In the period between her fourth and fifth birthdays, much of the clinical material had to do with playing that we were throwing things out of the window . This seemed to be the child 's attempt at mastery over her traumatic experi- ences . In fact, we did throw out toys and run outside to get them and soothe them and make sure they were all right. Over time, the play had moved from throwing inanimate objects like paintbrushes, to throwing baby dolls . She then engaged the therapist in throwing the baby doll from one to the other on a staircase, with first Pam and then the therapist soothing the baby, asking her if she was all right and comfort- ing her. In later sessions Pam began to use make-up and paint to paint her face and that of the therapist . In these sessions she would become very distressed and would use dark, thick make-up or watercolor paints to paint her own face and hands, and the therapist's and those of her foster mother, in an urgent attempt to communicate something . These ses- sions were frightening for her and for the therapist . What had been comfortable, related, engaged treatment sessions with a child who seemed to be maturing well and to be functioning well in kindergarten, family and church school, had turned into tense, anxiety ridden hours with a child who seemed compulsively to paint her face, to stand with the therapist in front of a mirror to deny that she was herself, and seem to beg to have words said that would help to make sense of what she was experiencing internally. Other examples suggest that this child was forming distinct selves that could evolve into MPD phenomena . On one occasion an adult acquaintance saw the child "change into another person ." Pam was visiting in the neighbor's home when the adult asked her to wait in one room while she put the baby to bed . The woman stated that when she did this she looked at the child who seemed to have changed . The 146 adult described it as making the hair on the back of her neck stand on end . The look, she said, was one of rage and hatred. The adult was stirred up enough to call attention to this event to Pam's foster mother and to ask to have the therapist explain what had happened . The therapist saw a similar event when the little girl turned into a "scary bear"- roaring and for all the world truly a frightening beast and not the child the therapist knew, totally unable to respond to the attention or intrusion of anyone . The therapist spoke to the child inside the bear, using her name, to recall her to herself. CASE ILLUSTRATION TWO A contrast to this case example is one of a child abused in similarly intense ways at a similarly young age, but for whom consistent empathic mothering, separation from the abuser, and early identification and treatment seemed to have led to a more favorable outcome. This child is the offspring of a mother who qualifies on level 3, highly probably evidence of DISS/MPD (dissocia- tion/multiple personality disorder) according to Braun's (1985) rating system for diagnostic certainty . His maternal grandfather and one maternal aunt also qualify for level 3. Two other aunts qualify for level 4, confirmed evidence of DISS/MPD . Three of the adults in this child's life entered treatment with the authors following the termination of the child's therapy. This child was brought for treatment by a mother who was desperately looking for someone who would believe that there was something seriously wrong with her two year old son, for whom she sought assessment and treatment . She reported that he cried unconsolably at diapering, that he manipulated his own genitals and his nipples, and attempted to stimulate himself on his mother's body, subsequent to visits to the home of the presumed abuser . He was terrified of bathing, had severe prolonged and frequent night terrors and was an unhappy, poorly functioning little boy. His mother reported trance-like phenomena at age two and again later at age four after an interruption in the treatment . During these events he would awaken terrified, crying, talking, and not responding to his name or to attempts to awaken him . He would cry for his mother as though she were not there, even as she was holding him . He would not remember any of these events in the morning, or would attempt to divert her attention from discussing them. He had further been reported to enter fugue-like states between ages three and four in which he would attempt to touch his mother's breasts and say things like, "I has to do it, I has to do it ." When the therapist first saw him in treatment at the age of two he presented as a tense, precocious, verbal, pseudo- mature boy with tremendous fears of broken toys, fear of playdough (which he called "soap - I don't like soap") and concern about mildly phallic toys . Of a Mighty Mouse doll he asked, "Why does it have a tail, why does it have a black tail? I hope it won't hurt me ." He was also concerned about "fooling around" and said that he hoped the elephant's trunk could help to fix things . He stimulated himself on the corner of a low table as he talked about the tail and showed DISSOCIATION, Vol, II, No . 3:September 1989 111ill II Jeff: in many ways that will not be detailed here the clearest evidence of sexual abuse of a very young child that the therapist had seen . After three years of therapy and suppor- tive advice to his already empathically attuned mother, this child was able to achieve cohesion . He exhibited a healthy sense of assertiveness and the process of internalizing ten- sion regulating functions had begun. MPD AS A DISORDER ROOTED IN CHILDHOOD Multiple personality disorder is primarily a disorder of childhood, although it is one with long-term developmental implications . Greaves (1980) in his review of the literature on MPD pointed out that "altered selves usually first manifest themselves in early childhood, as early as 2-1/2, and typically by age 6 or 8" (p . 587) . Fagan and McMahon (1984) con- cluded that " . . . multiplicity is established by 5 years or 8 at the latest ; yet it is almost never diagnosed before adulthood" (p . 26) . This suggests that it is useful to look at the adult manifestations . of MPD as survivals or long term conse- quences of a process that begins in childhood . This needs to be understood in the light of such phenomena as they present in childhood. Ulman and Brothers (1988) expanded the principles of self psychology to include an understanding of trauma . They contend that trauma results from real occurrences that have, as their unconscious meaning, the shattering and faulty restoration of "central organizing fantasies" of self in rela- tion to self-object . It is the shattering and faulty restoration of such archaic fantasies that are symptomatically mani- fested by dissociative phenomena . They found that these fantasies had undergone relatively little developmental trans- formation, which made the victims vulnerable to repeated shattering . Thus, the event itself does not hold the key to understanding the psychological meaning for the child experiencing it . They give examples of the lengths to which survivors of incest go to defend and compensate in order to restore their shattered fantasies . For example, a daughter's grandiose exhibitionism is fostered by early intrusions of incest by her father . Her fantasies were shattered by his brutal violence, which destroyed her illusions of becoming a famous performer . Efforts at defensive restoration were her illusions of having a powerful impact on the world while turning her father into an "evil genius" served as a compen- satory restoration . Her dual personality was explained as an unconscious effort, although maladaptive, to restore her shattered fantasies (p . 111). The tendency to approach MPD as a pathology of adulthood, and to search for comparisons between inte- grated adults and MPD adults risks having us miss the more important comparison between children who do become MPD in adulthood and those who do not . When we look at adult MPDs we tend primarily to contrast them with inte- grated or non-split adults and to seek the explanation for the disorder in differences between them . When we look at MPD as a disorder of childhood, the primary contrast is drawn with children in whom the process of integration has not been arrested. Adult MPD patients are not pure examples of the pathol- ogy underlying this disorder . The clinical picture in adult cases involves a great deal of secondary elaboration and alteration over time of the initial pathology (Kluft, 1985). Thus the study of adult multiples is limited as a pathway to discovering the fundamental process that goes into making up this disorder . In observing adult patients we do not see what MPD looks like in its purest form. In contrasting our clinical experience in the treatment of children with that of the retrospective data collected in the treatment of adult MPD patients we have noted that the emotional states of children as they are experienced in childhood are not always perfectly preserved in memory into adulthood . However, the presence of a child alter in an adult patient is so striking that it may lead us to believe that we are hearing from the real child . Thus we tend to overlook some significant differences between real children and the child- in-the-multiple . Also, the real child's external world is sur- rounded by self-objects the child can appropriately depend upon to provide the psychological functions necessary for internal structure building . In contrast, the child-in-the- multiple was created and maintained internally as a restora- tive measure, to provide a function within the internal system to cope with both the external and internal worlds. It is also important to the understanding of MPD that we learn how young children who will grow up to be MPD adults differ from those who will not . We need to learn at what point these groups diverge, and what the factors are that influence this divergence . Noting how MPD children differ from non- MPD children should be more fruitful than comparing adults with MPD to integrated adults. Kluft's (1984a) Four Factor Theory provides some help in this process by identifying the retrospectively described experiences that accompanied the memory of the splitting. We have found that these same factors, viewed prospectively, lead to a reorientation in the interpretation to be placed on these factors and on the treatment implications, especially the implications for the treatment of children. COHESION AS A DEVELOPMENTAL ACHIEVEMENT Kluft's (1984a) Factor I, capacity to dissociate, and our experience of treating very young children, suggests that unity of the self through time and space is a developmental achievement that reflects specific child-adult interactions subjectively experienced by the child prior to the formation of the nuclear self . The states of non-association and the distinct centers of experience that, in adult multiples con- trast sharply with integrated adults, are familiar and phase appropriate in young children . Gedo and Goldberg (1973) alluded to this by saying, "The utilization of the construct `self' has been hampered . . . by the inherent difficulty of grasping the subtle idea that the organization of the person- ality as a whole maybe an important developmental achieve- ment of early childhood but also by the semantic problems created by attempts to superimpose this concept on the tripartite model of the mind . . ." (p . 64) . From our experi- ence with young children in treatment, as well as preschool children and toddlers in other settings, it seems clear that the state of cohesion that we take for granted in normal 147 DISSOCIATION, Vol. II, No . 3 : September 1989 children after the age of six and into adulthood is an important achievement, arrived at only after specific kinds of experiences in the first three to five years of life. It may not be possible to find a place for the self within the scheme of the ego since ego is a concept at a diff- erent level of abstraction, referring to a narrower seg- ment of behavior and cogent with regard to behaviors that do not begin operation until long after the unifica- tion of the self . . . . If we apply the clinical discoveries of Kohut . . . to an expansion of developmental psychology, we may conclude that the phase of self-cohesion must be preceded by one in which aspects of the self are not yet unified . We believe that Freud was alluding to this state of organization when he postulated a phase of `separate instinctual activity,' or autoeroticism (1911) . (Gedo & Goldberg, 1973, pp . 64-65) Taking this into consideration, Gedo and Goldberg underscore the utility of Glover's conception of "separate ego nuclei" (1932, 1943) in the early phase of psychic life. Based on the previously mentioned distinction between the constructs "self" vs . "ego," Gedo and Goldberg modify Glover's designation to "nuclei of the self ." When we think developmentally and look to child pa- tients and their experiences in a developmentally phase specific way, we see the early MPD phenomena as being not so much a dissociative but a pre-associative disorder . In saying pre-associative, we refer to an early period (birth to 6-8 years) before the formation of a firmly cohesive nuclear self is established . The child needs to come to distinguish her/his existence and achievements from those of the primary caretakers, as well as to integrate and recognize experiences with various people, which have different emotional tone. Due to the complexity of these developmental tasks, the young child often fails to see these early experiences as part of the same reality . When, for example, we see how easily children are able to pretend, in play, to alternate between different emotional states, we are looking at separate nas- cent selves or separate centers of experience. It is our view that the phase appropriate existence of separate nuclei around which self-experiences can con- dense is more common than not in developing children . We hypothesize that split-off sectors of self nuclei seen in incipi- ent cases of MPD are related to a normal developmental analogue that precedes the establishment of the cohesive nuclear self. MPD AS PHASE INAPPROPRIATE SURVIVALS The third proposition is that incipient MPD and later periods of initial splitting, as well as some dissociative phe- nomena in adulthood are not primarily examples of coming apart (dissociation) but are phase inappropriate survivals of what were once phase appropriate and distinct centers of ex- perience and initiative . One hypothesis is that the adults in the world of the child who is to become a dissociator or classical MPD adult have not provided the integrating, consistent experiences over time and space that permit, 148 induce, and maintain cohesion. Every child requires consistent experiences of soothing of distress, confirmation of states of being, affirmation of achievements, pleasure and mastery in close enough prox- imity over time, space, and person to establish a unified experience of self . The absence of that cohesion in the pre- MPD child points not to a primarily cognitive event, but an internal, nonverbal, experience-near-event . Children do not come to know that they are a single person because they have been told this, nor do they know this automatically. They know it because they subjectively experience it with consistency over time and space by the psychological func- tions provided by adequate parenting. One brief clinical vignette illustrates the adult's acting in ways that enhance the child's sense of himself as being always the same person . The occasion was of a transient regression by a five year old boy, Sam, who had been left in another therapist's office in an agency by his mother while she went to spend an hour with her therapist . Sam "fell apart" in the way of an abandoned 8 or 10 month old infant - he cried, tried to pursue mother, finally fell on the floor oblivious to the other therapist's presence or that of other people . He followed mother as she went up the stairs and sat on the floor near the locked door of the office where she met with her therapist . Sam cried and screamed, and was com- pletely unable to tolerate the separation. What was striking about his behavior was that although Sam knew the other therapist, he was not able to attend to her presence in any way- he was in a trance-like state of rage and terror . The therapist sat on the floor with him and talked to him, without much success . The fragmentation state went on for at least twenty minutes . He began to integrate only when the therapist was able to catch something he was saying, to repeat his words, and then to remind him of his name, of what he wanted (his Mommy) and that he was still the same boy . What seemed to help him most to calm down and to become reintegrated was her saying the following words : " . . . And you're still the same Sam whose Mommy sings and plays this little piggy goes to market, this little piggy stays home and she touches your other toe and says this little piggy had roast beef . . . and the same Sam who misses his Daddy ." The feeling, in doing this work, was that of gather- ing together pieces of the child's self from various places, pointing them out to the child and weaving them together into a whole child . It seems that successively inducing his distinct experiences of "self-hood" at moments close to- gether in time and space helped re-integrate this boy. Games like This Little Piggy and peek-a-boo are much enjoyed and developmentally important family play with infants and in the child therapists ' clinical repertoire . They set up "slight fragmentation fears" for the child which allow him/her to test the as yet incompletely coalesced self in the safety of a loving/caring other, such that mastery of the fragmentation state can be achieved . This process occurs in normal child development routinely, although its impor- tance is often recognized only in cases where the self-object functions have been inadequate or absent (Kohut, 1971, 1978). The non-integrated state of having distinct nuclei of DISSOCIATION, Vol. [I . N . 3; September 1989 ALBI ' * it association is the typical state of the young child nurturing missing when Kluft's (1984a) Factor IV (inade- quate stimulus barriers) is present, that is the presence of a consistently empathically attuned adult providing self-ob- ject calming and protective functions, is the kind of parent- ing that all children require. Thus we ask not only why does the child dissociate, but what leads to the inhibition of the process of cohesion and the persistence and firmness of the split. This is an important difference. In Klufts (1984a) formulation, Factor III refers to shap- ing influences. These act to determine the form, character- istics and firmness of the isolated self fragments that persist and may even predominate in the patients we are discussing here . These shaping influences are important since they may determine how effectively a particular self fragment may substitute for the missing self-object functions (Factor IV). Kohut's (1971, 1977, 1984) "twinship" and "alterego" self-object concepts may be useful in understanding and distinguishing the role of "imaginary companion" phenom- ena in the developing self's manifestations of both pre- association and dissociation. Details of a particular event that may have been momen- tarily soothing protecting, or mirroring, and that may have momentarily dispelled the effects of a trauma, can be pre- served as characteristic of a self fragment. The momentarily protective influences are probably occasional or fleeting. Thus while effective to help the child retreat from the trauma (dissociate from bodily and psychic pain), they do not serve to enhance cohesion because they do not recur in times, places or with persons who are associated with other aspects of the child's life . Instead, their very effectiveness helps to reinforce the barrier between dissociated states and to ensure the persistence as the nuclei of independent self fragments. Thus a TV character who is associated with specific emotions or stereotyped ways ways of solving prob- lems may become the nucleus of a set of experiences that are markedly different from the helplessness of a traumatic sexual intrusion by an adult. When they work really well or when they contrast sharply with the severity of prior trauma, these isolated soothing events are reinforced and the barri- ers that isolate them from the traumatic experiences are firmed up . In the case of Pam (discussed above), a salient feature of the therapist was recreated and existed in isolation from Pam's most common experience of herself. These issues lead us to conclude that in addition to physiological components, MPD in early childhood results from a primary failure of cohesion prior to the establishment of the nuclear self. We may speculate that different degrees of dissociative disorders may be distinguished in terms of the degree to which cohesion had been attempted or achieved or experienced in rudimentary form in the young person before the overwhelming trauma. TREATMENT IMPLICATIONS While our focus here is not primarily on treatment issues, there is one important caution that should be made. The looser cohesion of young children is an important asset in childhood and not a sign of pathology and vulnerability to fragmentation that characterizes young children form an important avenue through which the child is able to turn to adults for self-object functions which will lay down profound supplies of self esteem, self-soothing, ambi- tion, values, internal warmth, empathy, vigor and organiza- tion which are the stuff of deeper and healthier adulthood functioning. When we take an adult-oriented approach to conducting child therapy, we are at risk for setting overly modest treatment goals. It is apparently possible to effect rapid and relatively lasting fusions in pre-associated children. If, as we suggest, these non-associative states are normal and transient in the presence of soothing adults (self-objects) then massive inter- vention specifically designed to reintegrate a split may be misplaced. It seems very possible that the young child who seems to have split will be able to make use of the availability of the self-object functions provided by the therapist to effect phase appropriate integration of the split off sectors of the self without the intrusions occasioned by hypnosis or more massive strategies to allow the natural, more gradual establishment of cohe- sion to unfold rather than to bypass the normally looser self- cohesion of the preschool and young latency child in favor of a prematurely cohesive but emptier child. CONCLUDING OBSERVATIONS The hypotheses underlying this paper's explorations and reasoning represent a particular paradigm for the under- standing of mental functioning, growth, and development. We appreciate that many alternative paradigms for the explanation of the phenomena we have discussed have been offered, and have been described astutely and eloquently. It has been our goal here to bring the perspectives of modern self-psychology to bear on the understanding of dissociative processes and dissociative psychopathologies, with the hopes that the application of this paradigm can further enrich the study of dissociation and the dissociative disorders a73 REFERENCES Berman, E . Multiple Personality: Psychoanalytic perspec- tivesInternational Journal of Psychoanalysis, 62,283-300. Braun, B ., & Sachs, R. (1985). The development of multiple personality disorder: predisposing, precipitating and perpetuating factors .P. (Ed Childhood antecedents of multiple personal- it.o(pp. 38-64) : American Psychiatric Press. Braun, B . (1985). The transgenerational incidence of dissocia- tion and multiple personality disorder: A preliminary report. In Kluft, R.P. (Ed.) ,Childhood antecedentsof multiple personality(pp. 128-150). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Fagan,J .P. (1984). Incipient multiple personality in childhood .Journal of Nervous Mental Disease. 172,26- 36. 149 DISSOCIATION. No. 3: September 1989 lN!klJ1i :a73a731! V Ur _11t71]I111111 Gedo, J ., & Goldberg, A . (1973) . The models of the mind: A psychoana- lytic theory : University of Chicago Press. Glover, E . (1932) . A psycho-analytical approach to the classification of mental disorders . On the early development of themind. New York: International University Press, 1956, pp . 161-86. Glover, E . (1943) The concept of dissociation . On the early develop- mentof the mind. New York : International University Press, 1956, pp. 307-23. Greaves, G .B. (1980) . Multiple personality : 165 years after Mare Reynolds .journal of Nervous Mental Disease,168, 577-596. Gruenwald, D . (1977) . Multiple personality and splitting phenom- ena : a reconceptualization .Journal of Nervous Mental Disease,164, 385-393. Kluft, R .P . (1984) . The treatment of multiple personality disorder; A study of 33 cases . Psychiatric Clinicsof North America, 7, 9-29. Kluft, R .P. (1985) . Childhood multiple personality disorder : Pre- dictors, clinical findings, and treatment results . In R .P . Kluft (Ed .) Childhood antecedentsof multiple personality. Washington, DC : Ameri- can Psychiatric Press. Kohut, H . (1971) . The analysis of the selfNew York : International Universities Press. Kohut, H . (1977) . The restorationof the selfNew York : International Universities Press. Kohut, H . & Wolf, E .S. (1978) . Disorders of the self and their treatment : An outline .International journal ofPsychoanalysis,59, 413- 425. Kohut, H . (1978) . Remarks about the formation of the self : Letter to a student regarding some principles of psychoanalytic research. In Ornstein, P .H . (Ed .), The search for the self Volume 2(pp . 737-770) . New York : International Press, Inc. Kohut, H . (Author) . Goldberg, A . (Editor) . (1984) . How does analysis cure?Chicago : The University of Chicago Press. Muslin, H . (1981) . Kohut's analysis of the self Cambridge : Current Medical Literature, Inc. Muslin, H ., & Val, E . (1987) . The psychotherapy of the self. New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers. Tolpin, M . (1978) . Self-objects and Oedipal Objects : A crucial developmental distinction . Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 33,167- 184. Ulman, R .B. & Brothers, D . (1988) . :l'heshattered self A psychoanalytic study of trauma. Hillsdale, New Jersey : The Analytic Press. 150 DISSOCIATION, Vol : September 1989