DREAMS AND DISSOCIATION THEORY: SPECULATIONS ON BENEFICIAL ASPECTS OF THEIR LINKAGE Stewart Gabel, M.D. Stewart Gabel, M.D., isAssistant Professor of Psychiatry at the Cornell University Medical College and practices psychiatry at the New York Hospital-Cornell Medical Center, Westchester Division,White Plains, New York. For reprints write Stewart Gabel, M.D., the New York Hospi- tal-Cornell Medical Center, Westchester Division, 21 Bloom- ingdale Road, White Plains, NY 10605. ABSTRACT The linkage between dreams and various dissociative phenomena has often been noted on an intuitive or clinical basis. Dream theory during this century, however, has been associated with and helped to provide the framework for psychoanalytic theory, not dissociation theory. In recent years interest in dissociation theory and dissociative phenomena has grown. This has also been true of the interest in dreams as understood from vantage points that dispute classical psychoanalytic views on dreaming and that emphasize a role for dreaming in learning and adaptive behavior. This paper reviews some of these issues in greater detail. It emphasizes the apparent linkage between dream phenomena and particular dream theories with dissociation theory. Possible benefits to dream theory and to dissociation theory when dreams are consid- ered within a broader framework of dissociation are discussed from several viewpoints. INTRODUCTION Freud Interpretation of Dreams (1953) is a landmark, serving both as a cornerstone of psychoanalytic theory gen- erally and of the psychoanalytic understanding of the nature and function of dreams specifically. Freud (1953) consid- ered this work to be perhaps his most important contribu- tion. For several decades after this monumental book was written, the psychoanalytic view of dreaming changed little (Blum, 1976), although some modifications were intro- duced as ego psychology and then object relations theory occupied increasing psychoanalytic interest (Arlow Bren- ner, 1964). More recently, self psychology has emerged as a strong focus of theoretical and clinical interest within the psychoanalytic community. With the theory of self-psychol- ogy has come a developing theory of dreams, initiated by Kohut (1977), but further developed by others, including Ornstein (1987), Greenberg (1987a) , and Fosshage (1983) . Freud towering presence within the mental health fields, especially within dynamic psychiatry, has been praised for its enormous contributions, but lamented because of its impediment to the development of other approaches that seek to understand unconscious processes (Bliss, 1988) . This lack of innovation has been commented on by Fosshage (1983) in his discussion of traditional psychoanalytic dream theory. Freud s influence also seems to have markedly im- paired the development of other theoretical frameworks, one of which is dissociation theory (Braun, 1988). The concept of dissociation, along with its modern day founder, Pierre Janet (1929) , may have been poised to assume a major position in dynamic psychiatry until being overshadowed by Freud development of psychoanalysis and his theoretical contributions in relation to conflict theory, defensive opera- tions, unconscious wishes, infantile sexuality and the like (Ellenberger, 1970). It has only been more recently, spurred on by several factors, including Hilgard (1986) experimental work in hypnosis and his neo-dissociation theory, as well as by a resurgence of interest in multiple personality disorder, that dissociation theory and dissociative disorders are widely discussed and studied (Kluft, 1988). Perhaps becauseof the lack of interest in dissociation theory during the decades of psychoanalytic preeminence, relatively little attention was paid to how or whether the study of dreams might be placed within the framework of dissocia- tion theory. This paper argues that dreams may be benefi- cially studied within the context of dissociative processes and dissociation theory. It traces some of the modern roots of what I believe to be the study and interpretation of dreams within a dissociation framework by reviewing the work on dreams of Morton Prince (191 Oa, 1910b) , a leading figure in early dissociation theory. It also reviews the dream theory of C.G. Jung (1954, 1960, 1964), who left the psychoanalytic movement to form his own Analytical Psychology. Jung understanding of the dream, and that of other members of the Analytical Psychology school (Hall, 1977; Mattoon, 1978) seem to fall within what will be described more fully below as dissociation theory. Finally, in the discussion, I describe more fully how dreams maybe understood within a dissocia- tion framework and within dissociation theory, and empha- size theoretical and research benefits to this conceptual approach. 38 DISSOCIATION, Vol. l11, No. 1: March 1990 GABEL HISTORICAL. CONSIDERATIONS Pierre Janet is widely credited with being the father of modern dissociation theor'. After decades of relative obliv- ion, the importance of his work is being increasingly appre- ciated and reviewed (van der Hart and Friedman, 1989; van der Kolk and van der I Iart, 1989). The major portion of his vast writings are still unavailable in English translation, and his possible contributions in a number of areas are only now beginning to be explored. From recent more general reviews available (Ellenberger, 1970; van der Hart and Friedman, 1989). as well as some of his specific writings noted below, it appears that Janet. considered dreams to sometimes reflect problematic areas his patients experienced and as potentially useful in his therapeutic work with patients. The present author is not aware of his having systematically formulated a specific theory of dreams as such and he does not seem to have given dreams the central focus in his work that major contemporaries such as Freud (1953) andjung (1963) did in their work and in their own lives. Perhaps this was partly because, unlike Freud and Jung, Janet was not a "good dreamer" and apparently did not remember his own dreams (Ellenberger, 1970). Janet (1929) describes certain hysterical patients' symp- toms, their somnambulisms, as being "dreams, " and his description of monoideic somnambulisms as involving "a blank in the general consciousness, which is represented by an amnesia, and as exaggerated and independent develop- ment of the emancipated idea" (Janet, 1929, p. 64), is similar to a modern description of dream characteristics offered to Reclrtschaffen (1978), a sleep and dream researcher more than half a century later, who emphasized the narrow focus and "single-mindedness" of dreams. Janet (1925) also discusses the dream as sometimes reflecting the work of repression, a central psychoanalytic concept, but writes that the dream is "the behavior of a sleeping man, and this behavior consists of the low-grade activations of tendencies that have been aroused by various internal or external stimuli acting upon the sleeper" (Janet, 1925, page 644) . The arousal of these tendencies occurs "in a very vague manner..." and depends on the "variations in the energetic charge of the respective tendencies..." which are "affected by numerous influences..." (Janet.1925, page 644). It is difficult to determine how much of the distortion in the dream " is the outcome of repression of one kind or another. " (Janet, 1925, page 644) . A more direct challenge to the psychoanalytic view of the meaning and interpretation of dreams from the point of view of a potential dissociation based theory of dreaming was provided in the early years of the 20th century by Morton Prince (1910a). Prince was well known for his work with dissociative phenomena and patients having multiple per- sonality disorders. He chose to investigate dreams by inten- sively studying a series of dreams in a small number of subjects. He focused his study on various states in the same individual : the normal personality, two different hypnotic states; and through automatic writing. He first obtained volitional memories from his subject in each state and he then attempted to obtain additional memories from the subject in the applicable states through the method of "abstraction," which he equated with Freud's method of free association, and which he felt was appropriate "for the purpose of resurrecting dissociated memories" (Prince, 1910a, p. 144). All of the details of this lengthy paper cannot be summa- rized here, but eventually Prince developed a view of dreams somewhat similar to Freud's in certain ways, but greatly different in others. Using the variety of memories he ob- tained ( that he believed were greater than could be achieved by free association alone), Prince arrived at several conclu- sions about the formation and nature of dreams. These include: The manifest dream relates to the thoughts of the dreamer's recent life, particularly (as revealed by hypnosis) those occurring in the immediate pre-sleeping state and those occurring during the preceding day. The " motive " of the dream was invariably traced back to "strongly organized systems of ideas which were deeply rooted in the mind of the subject, and represented her mental attitude towards her environment or the problems of' her daily life" (Prince, 1910a, p. 175). These "sets of ideas " were "conser ved " from the past, as indeed " every experience that is retained as a potential memcwv leaves a counterpart record in the neu- rones" (Prince, 1910a, p. 175). This record is commonly called "the unconscious. " The dream expresses the previous thoughts and experiences of the dreamer in symbolic form. Dreams are more than merely a patch work of previous memories, however. They have "a logical and intelligent design " (Prince, 191Oa. p. 175). This design is reflected by the fact that there often appears to be a motive going through the content of' dreams, similar to that of a storv constructed in a drama. Dreams seem to sometimes have a plot "as if an intelligence other than that of the conscious- ness of the dreamer had planned the development and foresees the outcome " (Prince, 1910a. p. 196). Furthermore, Prince argues that dream phenomena are similar to other dissociative phenomena in which subcon- scious (or, in psychoanalytic terms, "unconscious") proc- esses become manifest in conscious experience through symbolism. Examples include hallucinatory phenomena accompanying religious conversions and post hypnotic suggestions. Dreams do not appear to be unique in this regard. They represent one class of hallucinatory symbolism. Through his method and stud.) of dreams, using what he believed to be free association, with additional information provided by memories retrieved in dissociated states, Prince was not able to confirm certain basic tenets of psychoanalytic dream theory. He found no evidence of repression of ideas, of a censoring mechanism, of compromise formation, or of the imaginary fulfillment of infantile sexual wishes. He viewed the amnesia following dreaming to be similar to the amnesia commonly found for dissociated states in general. In summary, Prince viewed dreams as dissociative phenom- ena, similar to other dissociative phenomena. The dream results from sets of ideas or recent sensory experiences (especially those occurring in the pre-sleep stage and in the day preceding the dream). These ideas recur again in the dream, not as thoughts, but in symbolic representation. Neurograms (i.e. conser ved memories) reflecting these 39 DISSOCIATION. Vol.111, No. 1: March 199U thoughts and themes in the (Ircanrer's life stimulate associ- ated thoughts strongly related to the fundamental dream thoughts (motive). The subconscious (unconscious) proc- ess responsible for dream formation weaves together the original thoughts and associated thoughts into a dream consciousness whose format is a dramatic scene. This dra- matic representation symbolically conveys the dreamer's ideas and consists in part of "sensory hallucinations" and in part of thoughts. Why the particular action of the d eam is manifested as it is remains unknown. Prince does not arrive at an answer that he feels is satisfactory. If the unconscious process reflects a thinking consciousness, he argues, then. if the obser ver knew what this unconscious process was thinking about during the dream, one would know why the dream was as it was. If the unconscious process is determined by a "brain process" only, then one would still need to know the laws of this brain process 's functioning to answer the question. Prince does suggest however that dream interpretation based on these views may have therapeutic value in the treatment of hysterical patients. He suggests that stigmata of hysteria may be regarded as the symbolic manifestation of ideas from which they originate and which continue to exist subconsciously. These ideas may potentially be manifested in dreams. Ernest Jones (1910) subsequently replied to Prince 's study and conclusions. He was harshly critical and argued that Prince had not used the psychoanalytic method of free association properly, and "that far deeper memories can he recovered by psycho-analysis than by hypnotism.... " (Jones, 1910, p. 332), Prince had not come to Freud 's conclusions about the meaning of dreams because he had not used the psychoanalytic method properly. Prince (1910b) replied in defense of his approach and accused members of the psychoanalytic school of loose reasoning in the interpretation of facts and of a lack of broad experience with psychopathological conditions. He argued that psychoanalytic efforts were often characterized by dogmatism rather than by careful exploration. This type of exchange may have been similar to what Ellenberger (1970) has termed the "polemics" associated with the early days of the psychoanalytic movement. C.G. Jung (1961 ) , who was then a member of the psycho- analytic movement, was also critical of Prince 's study, argu- ing that Prince left the reported dreams unanalyzed. Wish fulfilhnen t and transference phenomena, Jung argued, were not discussed by Prince although they should have been. He, too, claimed that Freud 's theory was unharmed by Prince ' s repudiation. What Jung would have said about Prince's study at. a later date is not known. Several years after writing this paper (Jung, 1961), he left the psychoanalytic movement and formed the school that he termed "Analytical Psychology." While his own dream theory, to be described briefly below, went. much further along certain theoretical and clinical dimensions than Prince's study would allow, his later views on dreaming do appear to fit, at least partly, within a dissociation frame- work. Jung was an early follower of Freud who had also studied with Janet. Ah'hile the influences on his psychological theory and on his view of dreams, as expressed irr the theoretical framework of Analytical Psychology, were varied, his under- standing of the meaning and function of dreams does seem to fit, at least partially, within the framework of dissociation theory. This is not surprising considering Jung's personal background, interests, and experiences as described by Jung (1963), himself, and by Ellenberger, 11)70). who chronicles the history of dynamic psychiatry and the lives of its early major figures. Jung was the son of a pastor, and was deeply interested in religious and mystical phenomena. As a young man he joined a group which was involved in spiritistic experiments. Jung's own cousin, a 15 year old girl, was the medium for the group. She was reportedly prone to episodes of mediumistic somnambulism. 'I. his experience became the basis for Jung's medical dissertation. During Jung ' s own period of what Ellenberger (1970) calls his "creative illness," Jung developed methods of relat- ing to personified unconscious figures and conversed with personality aspects who became conscious to him through his methods of forcing his imagination to extremes and drawing out his dreams. For Jung, the psyche was composed of a conscious ego component and of unconscious compo- nents, personified in dreams. visions, and other phenomena to which the individual must learn to relate. Ellenberger (197(1) compares Jung's spli( off contents of the unconscious to Janet ' s concept of subconscious fixed ideas. Dream interpretation, according to the Jungian ap- proach (1954, 1960. 1964). will rink he reviewed briefly and in certain aspects here. An emphasis will be given to those parts of the theory that relate to dreams as presumably reflecting the workings of an unconscious component of the psyche which has an independent intelligence and aware- ness. distinct from ego consciousness. Jung believed that dreams arc prospective and adaptive in nature. They express, in metaphoric terms, the statement and message that is conveyed within the structure of the dream. There is no attempt to disguise unconscious content or to distort meaning. Indeed, in direct contradiction to Freud ' s view, the dream, as a reflection of an unconscious, autonomous source, stands in a compensatory relationship to consciousness. It is the responsibility of the conscious ego to try to understand the meaning of the dream and to develop a relationship to what is related through the dream in its own metaphoric and symbolic language. Dreams can and should be understood on the manifest level. Dream interpretation proceeds through a number of levels, but almost always involves an acceptance of the manifest content as making a statement about the dreamer, him or herself, or his or her situation. Like a story or drama, the dream proceeds to develop a theme with a conflict and then a resolution. The elements of the dream can be understood through amplification and limited association. Information based on wide readings in religion, mythology, fairy tales and the like are often helpful in interpretation, since the themes found in an individual ' s dreams are often the same or similar to those found in religious or mythological texts. The dream, as a reflection of an unconscious source, should be used to help the dreamer in his or her own 40 DISSOCIATION. Vol. 1I1, No. 1; March 1990 individual development. Jungian theory does not accept the notions of censorship. disguise. or distortion irr dreams. Extended free association is not used in dream i nfra rpreta- tion since this approach may reduce the benefits of the dream for the dreamer (e.g., in prospective and adaptive dimensions). For Jung, free associating to any of a rrurnber of images or ideas can lead to unc:cmrscious complexes. Dreams do not provide a unique or special approach in this regard. Early' childhood experiences are also not emphasized in the interpretation of the dream or as antecedents of particular dream elements unless the dreamer's associations bring these up. While space does not permit a full discussion, Jung 's approach has been influential for a number of modern non Jungian theorists, such as Ullman (Ullman & Zimmerman, 1979) and Fosshage (1983). Ullman has argued for the demystification of dream interpretation, dream work in groups, and the dream as an aid in achieving social connect- edness. Fosshage (1983) has elaborated on and contributed to the concept of the self state dream, which bears consider- able resemblance in certain aspects to Jungian approaches to the dream. DEFINITIONS OF DISSOCIATION Pierre Janet is said to have first used the term "dissocia- tion" (Ellenberger, 197{1). Janet described hysteria as "a form of mental depression characterized li the retraction of the field nl personal consciousness . (rid a trrtrlency to the disrociatinn and emancipation of the system of ideas and fi.rnrtion.s that constitute personality" (Janet, 1929, p. 332. Italics in original). One of the difficulties in studying dissociative phenom- ena and dissociation theory has been the frequent confusion about the definition of dissociation and its relationship to other terms, such as repression, used in mental health theory (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986). The current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders of the American Psychiatric Association (DSM III R, 1987) considers the essential features of dissociative dis- orders to be "a disturbance or alteration in the normally integrative functions of identity, memory or consciousness " (American Psychiatric Association, 1987, p. 269). \oung (1988) defines dissociation as "an active inhib- itory process that normally screens internal and external stimuli from the field of consciousness" (p. 35) . In pathologi- cal conditions, dissociation eliminates painful memories from consciousness. In normal circumstances, dissociation is nearly always present, screening out irrelevant stimuli, thus aiding the integrative functioning of the ego. Braun (1984, 1988) has conceptualized dissociation in what he terms a "BASK model. " The letters BASK stand for "Behavior," "Affect," "Sensation" and "Knowledge." These characteristics or processes are usually considered to he united over- lime in the normal individual. Dissociation may be defined as "the separation of an idea or thought process from the main stream of consciousness" (Braun, 1988, p. 5). Using the BASK model, Braun notes that dissociation may occur on one or more of the four levels (behavior, affect, sensation or knowledge) at a given time, while being congru- ent with the other components at other times. Within this BASK framework, fol . the purposes of this paper, dreams can be seen to fulfill the criteria of dissociative phenomenon, although they would not be considered ab- normal. Sleep, whether or not it contains remembered dreams, serves as an interruption in the usual stream of consciousness over time. Furtlrertnore, during the dream state, all of the RAShcomponents, as remembered by waking consciousness, ntav be distorted and/or separated from their usual states in the normal stream of waking experience. Sensation of self, for example, based on the dreamer ' s later report, may have little continuity with the consciously expe- rienced sense of self. Changes from ongoing, expectable, stable, behavioral, affective and knowledge (memory expe- riences are all common. DREAMS IN THE FRAMEWORK OF DISSOCIATION THEORY The value of considering dreams within the framework of dissociation theory rests not only on the possible validity of such a position, but also on whether such an approach aids in the understanding of other phenomena, dissociative or not, and whether it ser ves as a stimulus for additional theo- retical, clinical and research efforts related to dreams and dreaming. Michels (1983) has argued in another context that the value of theory building resides in the ability of new theories to generate additional relevant ideas, approach, etc. While dreams have long been felt to have dissociative char- acteristics by some theoreticians (Hilgard, 198(1), there does not appear to have been a recent effort to place them within modern dissociation theory, as defined earlier. Perhaps one reason for this rests with the historical lack of a clear theoretical or clinical framework for the interpre- tation of dreams that is widely felt to he consistent with dissociation theory, although, as I have argued, the Jungian view has several aspects that may be considered within a dissociation framework. The classical psychoanalytic inter- pretation of dreams emphasizes repression, censorship, and conflict among agencies within a relatively more unified system of mental processes than seems acceptable to or compatible with a dissociation based theory of dreaming. Dreams, however, are now being increasingly under- stood as beneficially interpretable according to what previ- ously were totally unacceptable lines in classical psychoana- lytic thought (Fosshage, 1983; Eisnitz, 1987; Glucksman & Warner, 1987; Ornstein, 1987). These new conceptualiza- tions may also facilitate the dream's being understood within dissociation theory (Gabel, 1989). Current conceptions of dreaming emphasize the legitimacy of the manifest content of the dream as a metaphoric, symbolic statement of the actual state of the individual and/or his or her psychological condition or problem. Current views of dreaming often also emphasize that the manifest dream proceeds, in the manner of a drama or story, to describe the dreamer ' s previous attempts to deal with a specific and focused conflict, problem or situation which may be internal, external (or frequently both) in the dream. At times, potential solutions or adapta- tions are also depicted symbolically and metaphorically in 41 DISSOCIATION, the dream; i.e., problem solving may occur. Despite variations based on theoretical differences, when dreams are viewed within the broad phenomenological outlines noted above, they suggest certain theoretical ap- proaches, questions and problems which I believe can be placed within and addressed by dissociation theory .. In the remainder of this paper, I will briefly describe several areas in which I believe the study of dreams may be enriched by or contribute to a greater development of dissociation theory. These dimensions (which often have considerable overlap) include: dream theory and dream research, developmental psychology and psychobiolop, brain-behavior relationships, learning theory, dissociation theory and dissociative phenomena, psychoanalytic theo- ries, and other areas of study. 1. Dream theory and research on dreams The description ofdreatns along the phenomenological lines noted earlier reduces or eliminates a variety of prob- lems created by the psychoanalytic model. These include the difficulty of studying or confirming on an empirical level such constructs as the censorship function, latent content, and infantile sexual wishes. While dreams, when accepted on a manifest level, also present difficulties for research, these difficulties appear more manageable empirically. Approaches to dreams aimed at defining thematic content, semantic structure, imagery, shifts in focus, etc. have all been made empirically and discussed in an extensive literature. Yet, the manifest content, phenomenological approach that is more easily studied empirically, and which is viewed within a nondistortion framework, and as an accurate por- trayal of psychological processes or of the state of the self (Fosshage, 1983; Ornstein, 1987) has a number of theoretical and empirical difficulties also. Briefly, the psychological truth, accuracy, or validity of the dream's description of self has been striking to many observers. Ullman (Ullman & Zimmerman, 1979) likens the portrait of the dreamer in the dream to what appears to be an image of him or herself taken by a camera far away in space. What are the psychological processes or mechanisms that enahle such astrikingly "ac.curat e " psychological picture of the dreamer to the displayed? How is the dreamer known with such accuracy, given the frequent defensive and distort- ing operations present in psychological life? Furthermore, how is it that potentially adaptive solutions and problem solving may go on in dreams when solutions to problems were not (or were not yet) apparent in the conscious life of the dreamer? Since Freud (1953, 1961) deemphasized problem solving as a function for dreams, traditional psycho- analytic dream theory did not have to actively address these types of questions. Dissociation theory may be able to address these issues, however. If dreams reflect a dissociated state within the individual, then interruptions or dissociations from the usual stream of consciousness in behavior, affect, sensation, or knowledge (the BASK system [Braun, 1988]) would not be unexpected. Indeed, feelings of strangeness, unreality, looking on oneself in the remembered dream from outside the point of view of usual waking consciousness would not be surprising. The dreamer, in usual waking consciousness, viewing himself or herself in the remembered dream, as if from a point in space or outside the system, might be thought of as having an experience similar to depersonalization or splitting in the continuity of the usual st ream of personal internal sensation of self, as described by Braun (1988) within the BASK system. Providing an appraisal of the dreamer (dominant per- sonality) which is broadly based and apparently psychologi- cally accurate and valid, can also be understood within dissociation theory. In this case one might consider as an analogy the various psychological functions of subpersonali- ties, including the internal self helper, wl has broad knowl- edge of the dominant personality and can help it achieve valuable goals (Allison, 1974). I do not wish to suggest that only dissociation 'Icon' can address these issues presented by what is emerging as a broadly based view of' dreams that emphasizes the manifest content of the dream, its accurate or valid description of the dreamer psychologically, and the description of possibilities available to the dreamer. I have suggested other approaches to understanding these phenomena elsewhere (Gabel, 1989) . What I do suggest is that dissociation theory provides pos- sible approaches to these questions on both theoretical and clinical levels. 2. Developmental psychology and psychobiology Dreams are most commonly reported from REM sleep (Gaillard, 1983), and in acomplete discussion of dreams, the association of dreams and REM sleep must be noted. As will be amplified later in the sections on brain-behavior rela- tionships and learning theory, REM sleep itself comprises a distinct phase within the sleep cycle, and it is through its possible association with dreaming in relation to learning, problem solving, etc., that one aspect of the case for dreams as possible dissociative phenomena is strengthened. From a developmental perspective, the characteristics of REM sleep change over the life cycle, going from about 50 percent of total sleep in the human infant to about 20 to 25 percent of sleep time in the adult individual. There have been a variety of theories related to the function of REM sleep that cannot be reviewed here {see Pearlman, 1982). Jouvet's (1975, 1980) work is of some interest in the context of REM sleep and possibly dreaming as dissociative phenom- ena, however. Jouvet has performed experiments with cats, in which there has been stereotaxic destruction bilaterally of part of the nucleus locus coeruleus, thus selectively suppress- ing the motor inhibition of REM sleep. Jouvet argues, based on his studies in which the still "sleeping" cats display dramatic and stereotyped behavior at a time when they would be expected to enter into REM sleep, that REM sleep seems to organize or program "genetically constituted or instinctive behavior " (Jouvet, 1975, p. 500). In another context, Jouvet (1980) has also argued that REM sleep may function to select particular genetically programmed behaviors, that is, to aid in the maintenance of variations in behavior. It does seem; in the experiments described by Jouvet, that the cats, although maintaining characteristics of sleep, have been acting in concert with internal visual stimuli, i.e. 42 DISSOCIATION. Vol. III, No. L Mardi 1990 GABEL perhaps dreaming. Jouvet (1973), in fact, does not distin- guish between dreaming and paradoxical or REM sleep. Schenck, Bundlic, Ettinger, and Niahowald (1986) have described human behavior in concert with dream imagery in a small group of patients who have neurologic dysfunction and who no longer exhibit the usual motor inhibition characteristic of REM sleep. Whether dreams work in asso- ciation with developmental changes in REM sleep to pro- gram or re-program behavior for life span changes needs further study. 3. Brain - behavior relationships There have been numerous reports (Putnam, 1984; Coons, 1988) describing biological, psychometric and psy- chological differences between dissociated personalities in multiple personality disorder. There has, of course, been longstanding interest in defining biological correlates or substrates fur mental processes. There is currently very active interest in defining biological variations that discriminate among normal abilities (e.g., verbal and visual abilities) and between psychopathological states or between psychopa- thological states and non psychopathological states. In recent work I have reviewed evidence that bears on the question of whether there is a relatively increased right hemispheric activation or efficiency in imagery, hypnosis, REM sleep and dreaming (Gabel, 1988a). This is a controver- sial area. While some evidence supports this view in REM sleep and dreaming (Gabel, 1988a), Antrobus (1987) and others have cited evidence contradicting this position. There is also controversy around the notion of a rela- tively increased activation or efficiency of right hemisphere involvement in hypnosis and in imagery, but considerable support for this view exists as well (Gabel, 1988a). Defining dreams as possibly existing within the .frame- work of dissociative phenomena and dissociation theory should facilitate the study of dreams (or REM sleep) in comparison with various dissociative states since dissociation theory offers the model of distinct states (e.g., hypnosis) which have been and continue to be described psychologically and biologically (Putnam, 1988). 4. Learning theory Some recent attempts to conceptualize behavioral, emo- tional and cognitive changes in dissociative disorders and dissociative states have focused in part on the concept of state dependent learning, which itself is related to neurophysiol- ogical changes during different states (Braun, 1984, 1988). In this view, memories, learning capacities, and informational systems may vary within the individual, and depend on his or her current state. In the case of patients with multiple personality.' disorder, particular personality characteristics would depend on the learning history of the dominant personality and particular subpersonalities. This view deemphasizes the psychological construct of repression and emphasizes state dependent learning to help explain the capacity for incorporating, integrating, and acting on given types of information. Dream theorists such as Breger (1967) and Greenberg (1980, 1987a), over the last generation, have cited experi- mental evidence from studies with dreams and REM sleep to support the view that dreaming is associated with possible problem solving or adaptational efforts. There is an exten- sive literature in both animals and humans describing ex- perimental evidence that is in general supportive of REM sleep's role in learning and memoy, especially (in humans) as related to emotional issues. It should be noted that there is less experimental evidence supporting a role for dreaming per se being related to learning and memory. Yet, the clinical theories of writers such as Jung, Ullman and Fosshage who have emphasized the manifest dream's description of the state of self, adaptations of the self, and problem solving approaches reflected in the dream, do seem to fit well on an intuitive level with the empirical results of studies on the function of REM sleep. Rosenberg (Rotenberg & Arshovsky, 1979; Rotenberg, 1984, 1988) has contributed an interesting theory to the literature which is illustrative of the potential for viewing REM sleep (and dreaming) as one state which complements another state (usual waking consciousness) in problem solving or learning capacities. On the basis of experimental work of his own with humans and animals, as well as the work of others, Rotenberg suggests that REM sleep and usual waking activity are in a compensatory relationship around the issue of "search activity." With its usual activities, waking behavior may become associated with a cessation of searching (for solutions, adaptation, etc.). REM sleep is, on the other hand, associated with "search activity." in states such as depression, conscious waking life has a relative "cessation of search." REM sleep then increases to compensate for this absence of search activity. Dissociation theory's interest in state dependent learn- ing (e.g. during hypnotic suggestion) and physiological changes during .states seems to relate well to these ap- proaches to REM sleep (and possibly dreaming), and may enhance and broaden our conceptions of the role of dream- ing in learning and prohletn solving. 5. Dissociation theory and dissociative states As noted earlier, there has been a dearth of experimen- tal work supporting essential concepts of traditional psycho- analytic dream theory. Rating scales that are empirically based have now been developed that can be used to assess individuals who are felt to possibly have dissociative disor- ders and to compare individuals with dissociative disorders and other conditions (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986). Review- ing items on such scales from the point of view of dream experience suggests that dreams may fit within a dissociation framework using these types of experimental tools, hut this hypothesis must be tested empirically. in addition to the theoretical interest now taking place within the field of dissociation regarding state dependent learning and physiological changes attendant on states as possible correlates of dissociative phenomenon, other work is progressing on the characteristics of the switch process between states (Putnam, 1988). This is an area that hears on the problem of shifting behavioral and psychological expe- riences of individuals with dissociative disorders. It relates to state dependent learning and possibly to the amnesia for 43 DISSOCIATION, Vol. Ill, No 1: March1990 different states exhibited by individuals with dissociative disorders. Shifts between stages of sleep, as is often the case in patients with multiple personality disorder, oc cui quickly. There are defined biological criteria (e.g. electroencephalo- graphic, electronnographic) that reflect change of stage characteristics to REM sleep, NREM sleep and waking conch- lions. Dreaming, as a fregttent concomitant of REM sleep, may be thought of as possibly exhibiting state shifts compa- rable in that regard to what may often be found in dissocia- tive disorders (and at times in other conditions). Theorists and clinicians in volved in the studs and treat- ment of dissociative disorders and dissociative states must of necessity define connnou characteristics or unifv?ing features of these conditions. Phenomcnologic and behavioral simi- larities between dissociative disorders and dreams are ap- parent (e.g., depersonalization, paralysis of movement, feel- ings of unreality). Another characteristic of dreams, em- phasized by Rechtshaffen (1978) has been mentioned ear- lier as providing some phenomenological similarity between dreams and dissociative phenomena. Rcclltshaffen empha- sizes the "single mindedness" of dreams, their characteristic focus on one main theme. Dissociative states. such as subper- sonalities in multiple personality disorder or in hypnotic states, are often desc ribed in a similar manner, as having a rather narrow, but focused thematic content. A thorough comparison of dreams with various dissociative phenomena, both those with known biological changes accompanying psychological state changes, and those currently defined on the basis of psychological state changes alone, such as depersonalization when neurological dysfunctions (e.g., right. tempo r al abnormalities) have not been identified, is war- ranted. Dissociation as a mental process has connnonly been linked with psychopathology, thereby possibly limiting its contribution to the development of a broader theory of mental organization. The origins and growth of psychoa- nalysis as a theory of mental organization rested not only on insights gained from psychologically disturbed individuals, but also from insights gained from and applied to an under- standing of normal individuals ' behavior. In The letepieta- tinn of Dreams, Freud (1953) emphasized that he used ex- amples of dreams from normal individuals (including hint- self) to avoid the criticism that the principles necessary for an understanding of dreams that he put forth applied only to the dreams of individuals with psychopathology. One of his goals was the development of psychoanalysis as a broadly applicable theory of mental organization. Dreams, a normal phenomenon, became a vehicle to understand mental proc- esses according to psychoanalytic principles that could be applied to everyone. Indeed, such was the importance Freud attached to his own work with dreams for psychoanalytic theory building that he continually returned to the Interpre- tation of Dreams when he felt uncertain about particular theoretical questions. Clinical, theoretical and experimental work with dissociative disorders and dissociative has led some workers in the field to argue that dissociation theory' may also have value in the development of a broader theory of mental organization (Beahrs, 1982; Watkins & Watkins, 1979-1980). The arguments put forth in this paper that dreams may be viewed front die perspective of dissociative phenomena bear on questions of whether dissociation may be conceptu- alized as both a normal and an abnormal process, and whether the concept of dissociation has value in the develop- ment of a theory of mental and personality organization. These questions are in part questions of definition and of emphasis. Several definitions of "dissociation" have been given earlier. There is no uniformity about the definition of dissociation when applied solely to pathological processes or when applied to situations in which dissociation also might be considered a normal phenomenon. The main f . cus of the arguments presented iii this paper relate to this latter question; i.e., whether the concept of dissociation may beneficially include a consideration of normal phenomena. Clearly the thrust of this paper is to argue in the affirmative. There is considerable support for this position. Van der Hart. and Friedman (1989) and Van der Kolk and van der I lart (1989) emphasize the value of understand- ing the concept of dissociation from abroad perspective that includes normal psychological processes, thus potentially contributing to the study of phenomena such as memory processing, state dependent learning and adaptive reactions to psychological stresses and conflicts. Current concepts of information processing in REM sleep (Pearlman, 1982) and in dreams (Gabel, 1987) have been noted earlier and also seem supportive of a "dissociation as normal phenomenon " concept. A broader neo-dissociation framework for under- standing certain mental processes in normal individuals has been proposed by Hilgard (1986) , based in part on his work with the hidden observer phenomenon. Psychological stud- ies with post commissurotomv patients have demonstrated parallel processing of information after surgical sectioning of the corpus collosum (Sperry, 1968). Studies of this type with "split brain" patients have led Gazzaniga (1985), a major researcher in the field, to develop a theory of mental orga- nization in which one language dominant, organizing, in- terpretive component of the brain constructs theories to explain attitudes. reactions, and behavior sometimes origi- nating in other parts of the brain that would otherwise be inexplicable to it. For Gazzaniga (1985), the brain ' s normal organization is into modules that function in parallel in a co- conscious system with one verbal and other non-verbal components, a view that seems quite compatible on a psycho- logical level with a broad definition of dissociation as a normal process that involves both parallel processing and "screening" functions (Gabel, 1988a). Yet, despite the suggestions implied by these various approaches that dissociation theory mightbenefically include normal processes and have utility for the further develop- ment of a theory of mental organization, there is no proof or evidence in a strict sense that dissociation as a concept should be broadened. The linkage which is suggested in this paper between dreams and dissociative phenomena has been argued on historical, phenomenological, conceptual, and empirical levels. Through the use of rating scales to assess dissociative experiences and ongoing research defin- ing and comparing dissociative and non dissociative condi- tions, this hypothesis can be further clarified empirically and 44 DISSOCIATION, Vol. III, No. l: March P1% the presence or absence of relationships established, but in the end the question may be as much theoretical and con- ceptual as empirical in nature. are discussed. REFERENCES 6. Psychoanalytic, and other theoretical and experimental work In the 1911 edition of The Inlerprelation o/Drearn.s Freud (1953) criticized Prince's (191Oa) discussion of the mecha- nism of dream formation cited earlier because of the lack of a dynamic explanation to account for diraar production. Indeed, dreams understood withit t the framework of normal dissociative phenomena suggest that the psyche may have autonomous or potentially autonomous components which may not relate to one another on the basis of conflict or defensive operations. This view challenges certain I adi- tional psychoanalytic concepts, but does not necessarily refute psychoanalytic concepts such as defensive operations, repression, and conflict as theoretical constructs within states or within the dream ' s mentatioll itself. It does suggest, as pointed out by Braun (1988), in the explanation of his BASK model of dissociation, that a crucial difference be- tween the concepts of repression and dissociation may he time. Lapses in the experience of the passage of time occur in complete dissociation whereas there is continuity in the perception of time when repression is operative. Dreams as normal dissociative phenomena, might also be described as experienced mental phenomena or states separate from the stream of usual conscious awareness. Prince (1910a), in discussing the mechanism of dreams, comments that "crystal gazers, " also demonstrating dissocia- tive phenomena, sometimes picture themselves itr the crys- tals, much as the dreamer remembers himself or herself pictured in (or verbalizing in) the dream. One aspect of dreams, as described (if not exactly? articulated as such) by theorists such as Jung, Fosshage and Ornstein, seems to be the ability of the psyche to monitor itself. There are of course possible dissociative and non dissociative theoretical con- structs to understand this process (Gabel, 1989). The study ofdreams within the context of dissociative phenomena may enhance the study of self monitoring and self visualization phenomena. SUMMARY Dreams have a long standing history of being considered dissociative phenomena and likened to other dissociative phenomena. Traditional psychoanalytic theory has described dream structure and function within a psychoanalytic model. There has been a recent increase in interest in dissociative disorders and the concept of dissociation from experimen- tal, theoretical and clinical viewpoints. This paper reviews psychological attempts in the modern era to try and under- stand dreams from what the author suggests may be consid- ered a dissociation perspective. It then discusses characteris- tics of several current dream theories and argues that dreams, as now understood in some psychoanalytic and non psycho- analytic orientations, may also be understood within the framework of dissociation theory. Various areas whose un- derstanding and study may be contributed to or enhanced by considering drearnswithin the context of dissociation theory Antrobus, J. (1987). Cortical hemisphere asymmetry and sleep me ntation. Psychological Review, 84,359-368. :Allison, R.B. (1974). A new treatment approach for multiple per- sonalities..lrn.eriran fatcnral of Clinical Nspnasis, 17, 15-32. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and slatislical ntrrnual of mentaldisorders (8111 edition - revised). Washington, 1)(:: At i t h or. Arlnty, J..A.,& Brenner. C. ( I961). Pyirhoanalytiir concepts and the clrurlrrral Ilremy. New York: International University Press Inc. Bcalws,.J.O. (1982). l'rritl' and multiplicity: Multilevelconsciousness of self irr hypnosis, psychiatric disorders, and mental health. New York: Rmmner/hazel. Bern.sleiu, E_I-1., & Putnam, F.V. (1986). Development, reliability and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of :Vinous and Mental Disease, 174, 727-835. Bliss, E.L. (1988). A rcexarniuation of Freud's basic concepts from studies of multiple personality disorder. DISSOCIATION, 1 (3), 36- 40. Blum, TI.P. (197ti) . The changing use of dreams in psychoanalytic practice. Inler'natlonal Jam-mil of I'nrhaanalssis, 57, 315-324. Braun, B.G. (1984). Towards a theory of multiple personality and other dissociative phenomena. Ps~rlr.iotdc Clinics ofNmthAnf uica, 7, 171-193. Braun, B.G. (1988). The BASK (behavior, affect, sensation, knowl- edge) model of dissociation. DISSOCIATION 1(1), 4-23. Breger, L. (1967). Function of dreams. puma] of.1&nrrnaiPsychology (Monograph), 72(3), 1-28. Coons, P.M. (1988). Psvchophysiologic aspects of multiple person- ality disorder: A review. DISSOCIATION 1(1). 47-53. F,isnitz,A.J. (1987). The perspective of the self representation in dreams. In A. Rothstein (Ed.), 7'1ae interpretation ofdrearns in clinical work. Madison. CT: International Universities Press Inc. Ellenberger, H. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious. New York. Basic Books. Fosshage, J.L. (1983). The psychological function of dreams: A revised psychoanalytic perspective. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 6, 641-669. Freud, S. (1953). The Interpretations of Dreams. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological SigmundFreud (Volumes 4 and 5) London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published in 1900) Freud, S. (1961). Some Additional Notes on Dream Interpretation as a Whole. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud ('Volume 19). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published in 1925) Gabel, S. (1987). Information processing in rapid eye movement sleep: Possible neurophvsiological, neuropsychological and clinical 45 DISSOCIATION, Vol. No. I: March 1990 D I l l 1 1 1 I O correlates. Journal eJ Nervousand Mental Disease, 175, 193-2{)0. Gabel, S. (1988a) . The right hemisphere in imagers :, hypnosis, REM sleep, and dreaming: Empirical studies and tentative conclusions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 323-33I. Gabel, S. (19881)) . Dissociative phenomena and monitoring of self: Experimental, clinical and theoretical considerations. Integrative Psychiatty, 6, 53-60. Gabel, S. (1989). Dreams as a possible reflection ofa dissociated self monitoring system.. Journal of.N `eivoru and Mental Disease, 177, 560- 568. Gaillard, J.M. (1983). Mental activity during sleep. In Ni. Monnier (Ed.),Fuactionsof the novous.systern (lbluno'4, Psychobiology), New York: Elsevier. Gazzaniga, hl .S. (1985). TheSocialBrairn. Discovering the networks ophe mind. New York: Basic Books Inc. Glucksman, M.I., Varner, S.L. (1987). Dreams in new perspective. The royal road revisited. New York: Haman Sciences Press, Inc. Greenberg, R. (1980). hreaming and memory. In E. Hartmann (Ed.), Sleep and Dreaming. International Psichcatrt : Clinics, 7, 258. Boston: Little. Brown and Company. Greenberg, R. (I987a). Self-psychology and dreams: The merging ofdiffererrtper'spect.ives.I's}'rltca-trir theUniversity M Ottawa, 12, 98-102. Greenberg, R. (198Th). On the importance of wearing two hats: Dream researcher and less cdcoanalvst. Joilnnal of the .1nu:n ran Acad- emy of Psychoanalysis. 15. 321-330. Hall, J.A. (1977). Clinical uses of dreams: Jungian intopretntions and enactments. New York: Grunt and Stratton. Hilgard, E.R. (1986) . Divided con scioosness: Multiplecontrols in human thought and action (expanded edition). New York: John Wiley and Sons. Janet, P. (1929). The majorsymptoms of hysteria (second edition with new matter). New York: The MacMillan Company. Janet, P. (1925). Psychological healing: A historical and clinical study (Volumes 1 and 2). (E. & C. Paul, Trans.). New York: The MacMil- lan Company. Jones, E. (1910). Remarks on Dr. Morton Prince's article: "The mechanism and interpretation of dreams." Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 5, 328-336. Jouvet, M. (1975). The function ofdrearning: A neurophysiologist's pointofview. In M.S.Gazzanigo and C. Blakem ore (Eds.), Handbook of psychobiology.New York: Academic Press. Jouvet, M. (1980). Paradoxical sleep and the nature-nurture con- troversy. In P.S. McConnell, G.J. Boer, H.J. Romign, N.E. van dePoll, and M.A. Corner (Eds.), Adaptive capabilities of the nervous system: (Volume 53). New York: Elseview/ North - Holland: Biomedical Press. Jung, C.G. (1954). Collected works ofC.G.Jung, Bolligen Series XX. The Development of Personality (Volume 17). New York: Pantheon Books, Inc. (Original work published in 1934) Jung, C.G. (1960) . Collectedworksof C.G.Jung, Bolligen Series XX. The Structure and D'inacccics oft he Psyche(Volume 8). New York: Pantheon Books, Inc. (Original work published as General aspects of dream psychology 1 19341 and On the nature of dreams 119451) .Jung, C.G. (1961) . Collected works of C. G. Jung, BolligenSeriesXX. Freud and Psychoanadysis (Volume 4), New York: Pantheon Books, Inc. (Original work published in 1911 as "Morlou Prince, `The Mecha- nism and Interpretation of Dreams': A Critical Review") Jung, C.G. (1963). Memories, dreains, reflertimr.s. New York: Pantheon Books. Jung, C.G. (1964). Collected works of C. G.Jung, Bolligen Series XX. Civilization in Transition (Volume 10), New York: Pantheon Books, Inc. (Original work published in 1934 as The meaning ofpsychology.for modern reran) Kluft, R.P. (1988). Editorial: A new voice for a new frontier. DISSOCL-IT1ON 1(1), 2-3. Kohut, H. (1977). Restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press. Nlatoou, M.A.. (1978). Applied dreams analysis: .4 Jungian approach. Washington, DC: V.H. Winston and Sons. Michels, R. (1983). The scientific and clinical functions of psycho- analytic theory. In A. Goldberg (Ed.): The future of psychoanalysis: Essaysin honor of Ileinz Kohut. New York: International Universities Press, Inc. Ornstein, P.H. (1987). On self-state dreams in the psychoanalytic treatment process. In A. Rut]tstcin (Ed.),The nu tetpretationof therms in clinical work. Madison, CT: International Universities Press. Pearlman, C.A. (1982). Sleep structure variation and performance. In W.B. Webb (Ed.) , York: John Wiley and Sons, lard. Prince, M. (1910a). The mechanism and interpretation of dreams. Journal. of 139-195. Prince, M. (191ob). The mechanism and interpretation of dreams- A reply to Dr. Jones. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 5, 337-353. Putnam, F.W. (1984). The psvchophysiologic investigation of multiple: personality disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of -North America, 7, 31-39. Putnam, F.W. (1988). The switch process in multiple personality disorder. DISSOCIATION, 1(1) , 24-32. Rechtshaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1, 97-109. Rotenberg, V.S., Arshavsky, V.V. (1979). REAM sleep, stress and research activity. Waking and Sleeping, 3, 235-244. Rotenberg, V.S. (1984). Search activity in the context of psychoso- matic disturbance of brain monoamine and REM sleep function. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 19, 1-15. Rotenberg, V.S. (1988). Functional deficiency of REM sleep and its role in the pathogenesis of neurotic and psychosomatic disturbances. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 23, 1-3. 46 DISSOCIATION, Vol. GABEL Schenck, C.H., Bundlie, S.R., Ettinger, M.G., Mahowald, NLW. (1986). Chronic behavioral disorders of human REM sleep: A new category of parasomnia. Sleep, 9, 293-30S. Sperry, R.W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in con- scious experience. American Psychologist, 23, 723-733. Ullman, A1., & Zimmerman, N. (1979). 11. ieng with dreams, Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher, Inc. van der !ha, O., K Friedman, B. (1989).A reader's guide to Pierre Janet on dissociation: A neglected intellectual heritage. DISSO- CIATIO.N 2, 3-16. van der Kolk, B.A., van tier Hart, O, (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Ps ychiatry, 146. 1530-1540. Young, W.C. (1988). Psychodynamics and dissociation: All that switches is not split. DISSOCIATION, 1(1), 33-38. Watkins,J.G., Watkins. H.H., (1979-1980). Ego states and hidden observers. Journal of Altered Slates of Consciousncs.s. 5, 3-18. 47 DISSOCIATION, Vol. III, N. h March 19911